[From Bruce Abbott (2004.11.30.1300 EST)]
I’ve been doing a bit of thinking about sea slugs (doesn’t everybody?
(:–> ).
Eric Kandel of Columbia University and others have spent the past couple
of decades studying the physiological mechanisms of learning in the sea
slug, aplysia. For such studies, aplysia offers several
advantages. Its behavior can be modified by experience (that is, it
can learn), its nervous system is relatively simple, its neurons are
large enough to sample the electrical and chemical changes that occur
within them, and the “wiring diagram” of each animal is
identical to that of all the others. Consequently, one can map out the
mechanisms responsible for aplysia’s various behaviors and
identify what changes during learning.
The simplest forms of learning are represented by the processes of
habituation and sensitization, which involve changes in the intensity of
a reflexive response to a given stimulus. In habituation, repeated
presentations of the stimulus at relatively short intervals lead to a
reduction in the strength of the response, perhaps to the point of
disappearance. In sensitization, the reflexive response to a
stimulus becomes stronger after a different, noxious stimulus is
presented elsewhere on the organism’s body.
In aplysia, habituation and sensitization have been investigated
in a defensive reflex. The top of the animal’s body houses a gill
structure and a fleshy siphon within the mantle. Touching the siphon
triggers a rapid contraction of muscles within the mantle walls,
contracting the gill and siphon and covering them with the mantle. After
a few seconds, the muscles slowly relax to expose the gill and siphon
again. Habituation can be demonstrated by touching the siphon every
10 seconds or so. Gradually the muscle contractions become weaker
until the response fails to occur at all. Sensitization can be
demonstrated by delivering a mild shock to the head. If the
gill-withdrawal reflex has not first been habituated, then subsequent
touching of the siphon produces a stronger than normal contraction of the
mantle muscles. If the response has been habituated, delivery of
the shock to the head immediately restores it. (In that case the
sensitization process is referred to as dishabituation.)
Kandel and his colleagues were able to trace out the neurological circuit
responsible for the gill-withdrawal reflex. Sensory neurons that provide
the sense of touch for the siphon synapse with motor neurons in the
abdominal ganglion. There is also a second pathway whereby axon
branches from each sensory neuron synapse with an interneuron, which in
turn synapse with the same motor neuron that receives a direct synaptic
connection from the same sensory neuron. Touching the siphon stimulates
the sensory neurons to produce action potentials, which trigger the
release of a neurotransmitter at the synapses with the interneurons and
motor neurons. The interneurons produce their own action potentials,
triggering the release of neurotransmitter at their synapses with the
motor neurons. The motor neurons fire action potentials, which then
stimulate contraction of the mantle muscles.
So what changes during habituation? It turns out that the amount of
neurotransmitter released from the axon terminals of the sensory neurons
diminishes, thereby reducing the postsynaptic potentials within the
interneurons and motor neurons, making it less likely that the latter
will reach their firing thresholds. Furthermore, the diminished
release of neurotransmitter is due to the inactivation of calcium (Ca++)
channels in the sensory neuron axon terminals. The process whereby
neurotransmitter is released requires an influx of Ca++ ions.
Sensitization of the same reflex occurs because sensory neurons that
detect the shock have axon terminals on the axon terminals of the
sensory neurons of the reflex circuit. Release of neurotransmitter at
these terminals increases the number of calcium channels that will open
when the sensory neurons of the siphon are stimulated, thus increasing
the release of neurotransmitter by these neurons. If the response
had been habituated, this undoes the deactivation of calcium channels
produced during habituation.
In thinking about these processes, it occurred to me that these processes
labeled habituation and sensitization actually represent changes in the
gain of the control system that is attempting to keep the perception of
siphon-touching near zero. Thus, what we have here is a nice
example of a system that automatically adjusts its gain. When the
stimulus is innocuous and repeats regularly, the gain gets turned
down. Under these conditions, whatever is touching the siphon
(perhaps a bit of seaweed waving to an fro in the surf) does not present
any danger to the animal’s delicate gill structure, and the gain can be
safely dialed down to conserve energy and allow respiration to continue.
Painful stimulation arising anywhere would indicate that something
potentially dangerous to the gill may be present and under this condition
a strong protective contraction of the mantle to protect the gill would
be appropriate.
These changes occur as a result of certain experiences and in that sense
represent a simple form of learning, but do not produce a reorganization
of the animal’s nervous system in the sense of adding or subtracting
synapses.
Quite a bit more work has been done with aplysia since Kandel’s
pioneering studies, and researchers today are using this animal to study
the physiological mechanisms of classical conditioning and (even more
recently) instrumental conditioning. Anyone interested in learning
more about this can simply enter “aplysia” and perhaps other
keywords such as “habituation” in a search engine (I prefer
Google) to bring up a treasure trove of information about this
research.
Bruce A.