Collective Behaviour

Angus Jenkinson (2018-01-02)

Kent

Thank you for these remarks. I am responding to the specific passage about modelling, and how and why you have turned to it.

I do not have reservations about modelling as such .
It is an important and valid way of approaching understanding. This also very much includes mathematical modelling. Any reservations that I have relate practically to the nature of the model itself. Following good cybernetic practice, a model is only capable
of giving a true representation of the variety that it models and the problem is when people extrapolate beyond the level of explanation that the model is able to give from its own design. Worse is when they discount other modes of knowing. But modelling is
an excellent tool and I have used it myself many times in different ways over many years. Moreover I have designed many thinking tools, methods of analysis and design, that have been widely used by practitioners, each of which is a form of model. My aim has
been to develop what I call archetypal models, i.e. models that embrace a wide variety of concrete differentiated instances.

What I have also found personally helpful as another route for understanding PCT is a method that I have been involved in for some decades. It could
be called personal experience, but the specific aspect of it is the ability to enter consciously into personal activities of thinking, feeling and will. With respect to the last, and the bodily activities of movement, sensing, and so forth, I met a dance
practice and movement practice back in the 1970s called Eurythmy when I was a teacher and at the time it was part of the curriculum for the pupils, so I had to take part with the Eurythmy teacher. This means that a purposefully controlled awareness directed
behaviour has been part of my world for decades: what PCT brought was a precision of operational explanation. That’s really why the activity was part of the curriculum: it brought conscious awareness – as well as practical self-conttrol – to the processes
whereby the imaginnatively conceived goal is realised in movement.

Over time I also came to appreciate the way in which the normally entirely unconscious realm of our physical activity can be rendered more conscious.
I mean that in the normal case we walk along the road with very little awareness of the actual activity of walking. We are not aware of what our feet are actually doing, nor the whole movement and flow through the limbs. We know it may not even be particularly
conscious of the processes of observing the pavement that we are navigating. It is easy to see in others on the street. This whole realm of what is called Will, the conative, and which is expressed in behaviour, is normally one in which we more or less go
to sleep, it is rather unconscious. Anyway, the upshot is that as a supplement to other forms of thinking and analysis, I can to some extent directly observe PCT in my own behaviour. I also find such experiments as the rubber band exercise compelling.

···

…â………………………………………………………………………….

Angus Jenkinson

On 27/12/2017, 01:39, “McClelland, Kent” MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU wrote:

From Kent McClelland (2017.12.26 16:30)

KM: My reason for getting involved in mathematical modeling was that the dynamics of control systems in action and interaction weren’t obvious to me when I first encountered PCT, and so it took a lot of playing around with models for me
to get a clear sense of how control systems work. Maybe a person can gain a deep understanding of PCT without trying out any mathematical models, but some hands-on tinkering with models really helps, in my experience.

If you’ve made an intellectual commitment, as I have, to the proposition that the dynamics of control systems are our best conceptual model for understanding the dynamics of human behavior, then you need to have a thorough knowledge of
how control systems actually work in order to know what to look for in examining empirical instances of human behavior. I don’t pretend that I’ll ever be able to model a strip of human interaction in all its real-time complexity, but some basic modeling seems
really helpful for cutting through some of that complexity.

I would say that the reason for using a model of any kind—mathematical or conceptual—is to highlight significant pattterns that might be possible to pull out of the messy complexity of an empirical phenomenon. Of course, models can never
be more than abstractions and simplifications of the reality to which they are applied, but for scientists (and folks like me who try to think scientifically) mathematical models are particularly useful because they provide such a compact and rigorous way
of communicating scientific descriptions of empirical phenomena. In comparison, ordinary language tends to be fuzzy and imprecise.

Does this speak to your reservations about modeling, Angus?

Kent

…………………………………………………………….

Angus Jenkinson

On 10/10/2017, 23:42, “McClelland, Kent” MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU wrote:

From Kent McClelland (2017.10.09.1500 EDT)

Re: Angus Jenkinson (2017-09-26. 23.57), Martin Taylor 2017.09.27.10.40

HI Angus and Martin,

At long last, I’m back in Grinnell and have had some time to think about the questions you asked, Angus. I’ll start with a response to that post
and treat Martin’s comments in another.

AJ: Thank you Kent, I am very grateful to you for sharing this.

I have started studying your paper and I can see the elegance of having a mathematical model for the purposes. But also analytical thinking. I regard it
ass a major contribution. More of which later…

KM: Thanks!

AJ: Although I have worked with conflict, it is not a speciality of mine. So what follows is an enquiry.

KM: My focus on conflict has led me to see it as a ubiquitous feature of social interaction, just as cooperation. collaboration, or coordination of one kind or another occur in almost every interaction. I think
of conflict and cooperation, then, as two varieties of the more general phenomenon of collective control. So, I’d say that anytime you study social interaction you’re implicitly dealing with conflict, and cooperation, too, of course.

KM: Granted that most everyday interactions between people don’t take the form of a conflict escalation spiral, like those described by Collins and analyzed in my paper. The reason that most conflicts don’t get
out of hand, as I see it, is that everyday interactions nearly always require collective control at a variety of perceptual levels simultaneously, and while conflict may be occurring at one perceptual level, cooperation more typically occurs other levels of
control. Thus, for us to do a comprehensive analysis of any given social interaction, we have to think in terms of the participants’ collective control of perceptions on a hierarchy from low-level perceptions of the shared physical environment to the middle-level
perceptions involved in language and communication, to high-level perceptions, such as norms or social identities.

KM: Some examples: Two sports teams are in conflict over which team will win the game, but they cooperate at low levels of perceptual control by playing together on the same field or pitch, using the same ball,
etc., and at higher levels of control by adhering to the perceptual principles defining the rules of the game. In most contests, the conflict between the teams doesn’t ever escalate into unbridled conflict. However, sometimes a member of one team commits what
appears to be a deliberate foul that injures a player on the other team (or just humiliates him). A series of tit-for-tat physical aggressions may then occur as the teams seek to even the moral score, and we see an example of the familiar conflict-escalation
pattern.

KM: Two people argue with each other. Cooperative low-level collective control nevertheless occurs as they use a common language and orient their bodies to face each other (although at these low perceptual levels
some of their physical gestures may symbolize threats of violence to the other). While everyday arguments commonly result in patterns of conflict escalation, as the participants trade increasingly cutting insults, for instance, most arguments don’t degenerate
into fisticuffs. A typical reason for this restraint may be that the participants are cooperatively controlling some higher-level perception, perhaps a mutual friendship or relationship that they want to preserve. Another possibility is that both of them recognize
and adhere to higher-level perceptions of norms that prohibit doing violence to another person or prescribe penalties for physical aggression.

KM: In general, the conflict-resolution literature suggests that one good way to resolve a conflict is to focus the combatants’ attention on some goal that they both share, or in other words to get them to cooperatively
control some higher-level perception, rather than conflictively controlling the low-level perceptions that they are fighting about. Interestingly, the MOL method of therapy operates on a similar principle of directing the attention of a client caught in an
internal conflict to some higher-level perception connected to the two lower-level perceptions in conflict, which then enables the client to prioritize which of the incompatible lower-level perceptions to control, or do something else entirely.

KM: Another example: A work team or a committee work together to complete a project or reach some other common goal. The overall tenor of the interaction is cooperative, but along the way lots of little conflicts
pop up about the best means toward reaching their goals. If the group is working together effectively, these conflicts are handled by talking these conflicts out with each other or else deferring to a boss whose word is taken as authoritative and who tells
them what to do. The basic purpose of verbal communication, in my view, is to enable people to align the reference values to be used for their collective control of perceptions. What communication communicates, I would say, are reference values for the perceptions
that the speaker wants the listener to control. People who want to work together cooperatively need well aligned reference values for their common perceptions, and lack of alignment is tantamount to conflict.

  1. AJ: You say: “Of course, every approach to modeling relies on simplifications and abstractions, and degrees of realism exhibited by agent-based models will vary with the level of detail built into the
    modeled agents…â€?? You also say that it does not reproduce the detail of Collins’ explanation. Understood, but one of the cybernetic principles was that the best model was reality itself. Moreover, you comment that Collins does not give precise enough definitions.
    But part of the problem of “definition� is that it tends to reduce the variety of the phenomenon to a boundary defined by the definition. So I am interested in the degree to which you think that real life (sic) can be reproduced in a mathematical model? I
    return to this below…

KM: The highly simplified mathematical models that I used in my paper pertain to only a single level of perception. When multiple levels of perception are in play, as in the examples I’ve suggested above, these
simple mathematical models obviously can’t begin to reproduce the real-life complexity of cooperation on one level of perception and conflict on another. More sophisticated multi-level mathematical models might be able to do a little better job, but it would
take an enormously complex mathematical model to simulate the multi-level complexity of the real-life interaction. What mathematical models are good for, in my view, is to alert us to the basic patterns of dynamic interaction we might expect to see in these
real-life instances and also to provide a theoretically grounded explanation of why such patterns are likely to emerge.

  1. Building on that, as an illustration, you say:
  1. The other core variables in Collins’s model also lack of the kind of conceptual precision needed to support mathematical reformulation. Collins explicitly defines the variable atrocities as a matter
    of the perceptions of people on one side of the conflict interaction: “Atrocities are opponents’ actions that we perceive as especially hurtful and evil . . .� (2012:2). However, he does not offer any operational definition * that
    would enable observers to judge whether a particular action committed by one side is likely to be defined as an atrocity by the other*. My emphasis
  1. One of the conditions of conflict I understand is that the supposedly rational judgement is lost and people tend to find unacceptable, and perhaps extremely so, based on, let’s call it, a biased reaction.
    So what the person deems to be an atrocity might not be seen as such by an observer. That’s easily understood.

KM: Nevertheless, many of the violent atrocities that Collins describes would strike almost any observer as atrocities. When you kill scores of people, for example, that’s an atrocity in anybody’s book. My definition
of violence, from a PCT viewpoint, is the destruction of environmental feedback paths that someone else needs to keep their highly valued perceptions under control. When you kill someone, for instance, you render their physical body nonoperational, and the
physical body always forms two essential links in the circuit of pathways used by individuals to control their perceptions: on the output side, the muscular movements that enable individuals to manipulate their physical environments, and on the input side,
the sensory organs that report the effects of those manipulations. But violence can also consist of destroying physical objects that individuals rely on to control perceptions, like their houses or tools. It’s when you get to higher levels of perception, what
Pierre Bourdieu referred to as symbolic violence, that definitions of what an atrocity might consist of get more subjective. Is destroying someone’s good name, for instance, an atrocity? It’s certainly an essential link in the feedback path that people use
to maintain a positive sense of self.

  1. One of the rediscoveries of neuroscience is the role of emotion in decision making. I am interested in how the strength and roles of those appear in the model?
  1. You refer to reference values as memories (aka mental pictures). So when you say you “will take polarization to refer to the extent of differences in reference values�, I take that to mean that the
    (actively filtered and probably distorted) perception is compared by each party to something conjured from their own memories (forgive the unmathematical description).
  1. You also say, “PCT models show the output of the two interacting systems diverging, with one system, in effect, pulling in one direction to bring the environmental variable into line with its preferences,
    while the other system pulls in the other (see McClelland 2004, 2006). Hence, the variable used in my simulations is the intensity of the conflict, measured by the degree of divergence in system outputs.�Outputs are to the control variable of the other as
    I understand it.

KM: As Martin notes in his reply, one’s outputs affect the shared environmental reality, and the opponent controls a separate perception based on that environmental reality, but that perception isn’t necessarily
based on the opponent’s perception of the individual’s output. What happens in conflict is that a positive feedback loop emerges that links the two opponents’ outputs, the portions of the environment on which both outputs are acting, and their respective perceptual
control loops.

  1. So how do you process emotion in the model?

KM: Martin’s reply, directing you to Ch 17 in the second edition of Behavior: The Control of Perception, is a good start for answering the question about the place of emotion in this model. What I’d add is that
Powers argues that the experience of prolonged inability to bring a valued perception under control inevitably produces negative emotions like frustration and anger. In conflict situations, the actions of the opponent by definition prevent one from bringing
the contested perceptual variable into control at one’s own reference level, so the PCT definition of conflict implies that these situations always produce negative emotions. Emotion is just an expected part of conflict. The other thing I would say is that
because people are operating on several different levels of perception at the same time, emotional reactions do not preclude rational behavior. The two processes can go on simultaneously.

  1. How do you see the model contributing to practitioner work? And what other variables would need to be included? Is it more than offering a conceptual framework to think differently? Meaning, how would
    a version be used in Trump v Kim JU?

KM: The mud-slinging match between Trump and Kim sure looks like a classic conflict-escalation cycle to me. What my model does not do is predict the outcome of this escalating conflict, because it doesn’t take into
account the other presumably higher-level perceptions that these two actors may be controlling. Let’s hope that there are some perceptions that restrain them before they start pushing nuclear buttons!

  1. It seems that being able to prove similar results to positive feedbacks through negative feedback is very useful in rethinking what happens.
  1. It also seems to me it replicates what practitioners in conflict resolution do. They ask each party to see how the other party sees things. Negotiators teach the same thing.

KM: Good point.

  1. Might however the +ve and –ve bee mirrors of each other according to how one analysis the situation? One is dealing with a series of escalating outcomes that propel each other and the other a series
    of negative (internal) cancellations that also effectively propel each other? Your strength is to show the interaction, maybe?

KM: I’m not quite sure what you mean by the ±ve in the model. The value of the environmental variable [v] times what [e]?

  1. The paper also introduces some clarity in thinking about issues. This is a typical case in all model building and as long as one does not allow a spurious impression of detail to get in the way, can
    help. I like for example the parsimonious description: “From the PCT perspective, conflict occurs whenever the two sides use different reference standards in their attempts to control an environmental variable, and victory or defeat in the contest is represented
    by the extent to which side or the other succeeds in bringing the variable into line with its own preferred reference conditions�.
  1. I wonder whether it does not verbally – as a result of yyour thinking through the issues with the help of modelling – give the practitioner as goood an insight as a mathematical model?

KM: The model goes beyond the verbal description to show the dynamics of the interaction, something the verbal description can’t really do.

BTW: You mention: ““reflex arc� as conventionally understood�; which version of this do you mean? Presumably not the one that Dewey critiqued?

KM: No, I am talking about Dewey’s reflex arc, as well as the set of internal neural connections that provides the focus of most current neurological analyses.

KM: Thanks again for your good questions and your interest in my work!

Kent

—&nbssp;

A

On 24/09/2017, 20:20, “McClelland, Kent” MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU wrote:

[from Kent McClelland 2017.09.24.1735]

Hi Angus (and Christopher),

I’ve done a lot of thinking and some PCT-based modeling that relates to questions of collective action, organizations, and, more broadly, how society is possible.

The modeling I’ve done may strike you as pretty simplistic for your purposes, but the best summary of my work along that line can be found in a paper published in 2014 in the journal Sociological Theory called “Cycles
of Conflict: A Computational Alternative to Collins’s Theory of Conflict Escalation.” You can download a copy of this paper, as well as my other published papers on PCT, from my page in ResearchGate.

A much longer and more detailed, but strictly theoretical, exposition of my ideas about “collective control”, as I’ve called it, is contained in my chapter for the Living Control Systems IV volume being published
by Bill Powers’s sister, Alice. The chapter is called “Social Structure and Control: Perceptual Control Theory and the Science of Sociology.”

Unfortunately, the LCS IV volume has been slow to appear and still remains unpublished. I don’t know when it will be out, but the publisher has requested that authors not distribute any draft copies of the chapters
prior to publication, so I can’t share my chapter with you at this point. I’ve posted a summary of the chapter on ResearchGate, but it’s probably too condensed to provide any very good sense of my argument.

Martin Taylor’s contribution to the LCS IV volume may also be of interest to you when that book appears. I’ve seen some early drafts of his chapters, and many of his ideas were immediately influential to me as I put my own chapter together. He comes at the
question of how to apply PCT to social interaction from a somewhat different angle than I do, but his work is unlike anything else I’ve seen, and I think it will prove foundational for lots of future applications of PCT in this area.

Please send me some examples of your work if there are things you’d like to share. I’m definitely interested.

And thanks to all for the birthday greetings! I’ve had a fun birthday going with two of my young grandsons to the chocolate museum in Hamburg, Germany.

Kent

On Sep 24, 2017, at 3:08 PM, Christopher Paul Geddes geddes.chris@gmail.com wrote:

Angus,

Thanks for writing. I hope all is well. I have done some modeling in the past however I was kind of limited and now I am more focused on Systems Dynamics which is making me take more of an interest in modeling now
as well.

I would like to know more about what you are looking for if I can help. I like to think of organizations as entities that I can influence with technology so that things so the way the team wants them to go.

Christopher

On Sun, Sep 24, 2017 at 5:48 AM, Angus Jenkinson angus@angusjenkinson.com wrote:

[Angus Jenkinson, 2017-09-23.11.46]

Kent, Bruce Nevin has suggested that I should reach out to you in relationship to some work on modelling that you have been doing on collective behaviour.
I am interested, and working with, the behaviour of organisations and social groups and the relationship between the individual and the group. I understand you have done some computer modelling of this. Is there something you can share?

And happy birthday!

………………………â€â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦………………………….

Angus Jenkinson

Regards,

Christopher Paul Geddes

Email: geddes.chris@gmail.com

Mobile: +1-876-393-1366

Angus Jenkinson (2018-01-02)

Interesting, thank you. A few brief comments and…

A happy new year!

I have emphasized in bold the key ideas I am responding to.

···

On 01/01/2018, 05:25, “Bruce Nevin� bnhpct@gmail.com wrote:

[From Bruce Nevin (2017.12.31.23:25 ET)]

Kent McClelland (2017.10.09.1500 EDT) –/span>

Angus Jenkinson, 26.12.17. 15:00 Singapore –

KM: What mathematical models are good for, in my view, is to alert us to the basic patterns of dynamic interaction…

<

AJ: If a [mathematical] model is required to provide a rigorous analysis and yet the complexity of shared cooperative/conflict reality is too dense to model, in practice, how is the subject to be understood?

AJ: If mathematics is a form of language, as some argue, would it make more sense to say that is hard to achieve any description that does justice to the complexity of social interaction?

AJ: And in what language or thinking form does the “alerting� take place�

Angus Jenkinson, 29.12.2017 –“

AJ: On the alerting, you do not answer the question. Or if you intend to I find it interesting.

AJ: The models are basic and too simple to express reality, says Kent. As models they are patterns. But the alerting is to something more than the model shows, he says. Thus the paper
has both mathematics and verbal discussion. The verbal says, and model design implies, that an extrapolation is possible. But the model cannot show what the full pattern would reveal. (It would be another discussion to point out the danger of extrapolating
beyond what the model actually models.). If that alerting is to be understood in a useful way, but the mathematical extrapolation has not been done, then I asked in what language the alerting takes place. Do you say, No language, pure thought pattern?

But I did answer, as follows:

KM: What mathematical models are good for, in my view, is to alert us to the basic patterns of dynamic interaction…

BN:
The ‘alerting’ is in the form of mathematical perceptions of “the basic patterns of dynamic interaction�, facilitated or sometimes merely hinted by the language-like notational expressions with which a mathematical model is presented.

AJ: what is a mathematical perception? I think we are dealing with the perception of thinking by thinking, one of the noted distinctions between humans, I think, and all other life forms on earth. Anyway, I think this is describing the
process whereby one observes an activity form of thinking. That is the activity of thinking takes a particular form, such as a logical balancing. Thinking itself is able to observe this thinking form in its activity. This is the preliminary to all original
thinking and new concepts/intuitions/colligations. This pure activity of thinking observing itself – and I can refer you to an excellent PhD on the subject – then needs to take communicaable form. Symbolic logic and mathematics are both the means of assisting
this activity and designs for communicating and expressing certain thought forms in modes as close as possible to the original thinking activity itself.

Language is a rather rigid channel for transmission of information, and as its derivative (specialized for mathematical perceptions) so likewise are the various mathematical notations
and the language expressions in which mathematicians ‘read out’ such notations.

The transmission is by what we might describe as a socially structured evocation of memory associations.

AJ: Only when no original thinking, and therefore no real thinking is required?

As I see it, there are associations linking language and non-language perceptions.

AJ: I think this offers an interesting line of enquiry. What is a language and what is a non-language perception? If we include the hermeneutic line of thought, going back say to Perce, but it is really
much more ancient, then everything ever perceived is always the perception of a ‘text’. Another rough way of saying this is that nothing is anything until it has meaning and until it is phenomenon (has form, gestalt, significance, et cetera) and so is distinguished
from the seamless realm of perception.

Rather than Category perceptions, I suspect (and have proposed) that the associative links are probably perceptual inputs (both language inputs of non-language perceptions and non-language
inputs from language perceptions) as well as associative memory. Bill proposed that the nexus of the perceptual input links was in perceptual input functions at the Category level. I have written about my misgivings, but the proposals I have made are also
speculative and undemonstrated. Anyway, Bill assumed that the language input to a category would be a word, but the associations are not just between individual words and non-language perceptions, but also involve on the language side word-dependencies (between
an operator word and particular words in its argument, constructions of greater or lesser complexity, fixed expressions, etc.

AJ: Either mathematics only thinks in a restricted domain or its expression in present mathematical possibilities restricts its ability to convey the full complexity of “reality�.
Because all the mathematics today cannot capture all the world of hearing a lark’s song.

I can’t speak to limitations of mathematics,
but we surely don’t know all of it any more than we know all of the reality that we attempt to model with mathematical models. And
I think what you mean by the world of hearing a lark’s song has as much in it of human emotional response as it does of larks, or am I mistaken? We cannot ‘capture’ all of our experience in any form of language or language-like system. How much we ‘capture’ is up to the recipient as much as it is up to the speaker or writer. And that is true even of those who write to find out what they think (as
some have said). An interesting reflection, that.

AJ: Given that I was enquiring of Kent how emotion was modelled into his conflict model, it would be no surprise to find that emotion, or perhaps better put, the feeling response to the world, would most
certainly be included in this. And since I am a subscriber to the theory of ternary cybernetics, which builds this realm (denoted as imparity) into science as a necessary factor in understanding all system behaviour, and I take it to be a reality in human
behaviour, I have no problem so doing. In fact, I would argue that one of the greatest fallacies of classical science was to reducing motion to a secondary and unreliable feature of human existence. Certainly, people have “unreliable� emotional influences
on their take on the world and behaviour, but that is also true of their thinking. And I do not need to say that I am not alone in taking this line. But I also would not reduce the distinction I am making only to the realm of emotion. I predict 21st
century mathematics will find new ways of modelling/thinking qualia just as it did in modelling phase space (especially multidimensional descriptive space) and developing field equations. So that agrees with you that we don’t know the all of mathematics yet.

To paraphrase Korzybski’s pithy aphorism the model is not the phenomenon. And to paraphrase an equally pithy
aphorism of Sapir, all models leak . But by understanding (our perceptions of) the model we can more alertly perceive phenomena that are more complex than is convenient to be modeled. But I am only repeating here your paraphrase of Kent: “The models are
basic and too simple to express reality”. Rarely, I think, does a model or an experiment not rest on some prior selection of cases amenable to be considered.

Did not know the Sapir aphorism, like it, thank you.

We are agreeing. But the question I then ask is about the nature of models: is there a form of cognitive activity that resists these leakages?

That is in answer to your question

AJ: If a [mathematical] model is required to provide a rigorous analysis and yet the complexity of shared cooperative/conflict reality is too dense to model, in practice, how is the subject to be understood?

We must not forget that the models, the mathematical expressions, the language expressions, the social interactions that Kent is beginning to model, and the larks with their songs,
are not accessible to us in their full reality, only as perceptions, however extended our native perceptions may be by the perceptions that we control as instruments and by the perceptions that we control as models.

AJ: Here I am inclined to disagree with you philosophically. I respond (in my own enquiry) by asking how to know full reality. One aspect is not to accept the statement,
only as perceptions . Actually, perceptions in themselves yield very little. Our organisation is such that our ordinary senses give us percepts, which only become meaningful when united with concepts (obviously different terms might be used for this
statement). That leads to the epistemological question, what is the level of knowledge that is ideally possible to achieve by thinking? Is there an inherent limitation? Kant thought there was, but there is a rebuttal to his conclusion. If one further takes
the line the world is a monad, and therefore you do not turn percept and concept into dualism, then the process of our cognitive activity reflects a reuniting of an inherent unity.

You opened your email with:

AJ: Thank you for once again seeking to advise me.

Oh my. I’m honored that you think so (though the words from another might be mocking). Actually, I’m just trying to articulate my understandings, putting them out there to be corrected
if I misunderstand or amended if there’s a better way of articulating them. So please fire away.

AJ: of course, I saw the possibility of it being seen as mocking, but I assumed, rightly, that you would not be fooled.

Happy new year, all! May you control well all the perceptions that most matter to you.

/Bruce

Angus
Jenkinson (2018-01-02)

[Angus Jenkinson, 2017-09-23.11.46]

Kent, Bruce Nevin has suggested that I should reach out to you in relationship to some work on modelling that you have been doing on collective behaviour.
I am interested, and working with, the behaviour of organisations and social groups and the relationship between the individual and the group. I understand you have done some computer modelling of this. Is there something you can share?

And happy birthday!

···

…………………………………………………………………………….

Angus Jenkinson

Angus,

Thanks for writing. I hope all is well. I have done some modeling in the past however I was kind of limited and now I am more focused on Systems Dynamics which is making me take more of an interest in modeling now as well.Â

I would like to know more about what you are looking for if I can help. I like to think of organizations as entities that I can influence with technology so that things so the way the team wants them to go.

Christopher

···

On Sun, Sep 24, 2017 at 5:48 AM, Angus Jenkinson angus@angusjenkinson.com wrote:

[Angus Jenkinson, 2017-09-23.11.46]

Â

Kent, Bruce Nevin has suggested that I should reach out to you in relationship to some work on modelling that you have been doing on collective behaviour.Â
I am interested, and working with, the behaviour of organisations and social groups and the relationship between the individual and the group. I understand you have done some computer modelling of this. Is there something you can share?

Â

And happy birthday!

Â

………€¦â€¦……………………………………………………… ¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦.

Angus Jenkinson

Regards,

Christopher Paul Geddes

Email:Â geddes.chris@gmail.com

Mobile: +1-876-393-1366

[from Kent McClelland 2017.09.24.1735]

Hi Angus (and Christopher),

I’ve done a lot of thinking and some PCT-based modeling that relates to questions of collective action, organizations, and, more broadly, how society is possible.

The modeling I’ve done may strike you as pretty simplistic for your purposes, but the best summary of my work along that line can be found in a paper published in 2014 in the journal Sociological Theory called “Cycles of Conflict: A Computational Alternative
to Collins’s Theory of Conflict Escalation.” You can download a copy of this paper, as well as my other published papers on PCT, from my page in ResearchGate.

A much longer and more detailed, but strictly theoretical, exposition of my ideas about “collective control”, as I’ve called it, is contained in my chapter for the Living Control Systems IV volume being published by Bill Powers’s sister, Alice. The chapter
is called “Social Structure and Control: Perceptual Control Theory and the Science of Sociology.”

Unfortunately, the LCS IV volume has been slow to appear and still remains unpublished. I don’t know when it will be out, but the publisher has requested that authors not distribute any draft copies of the chapters prior to publication, so I can’t share
my chapter with you at this point. I’ve posted a summary of the chapter on ResearchGate, but it’s probably too condensed to provide any very good sense of my argument.

Martin Taylor’s contribution to the LCS IV volume may also be of interest to you when that book appears. I’ve seen some early drafts of his chapters, and many of his ideas were immediately influential
to me as I put my own chapter together. He comes at the question of how to apply PCT to social interaction from a somewhat different angle than I do, but his work is unlike anything else I’ve seen, and I think it will prove foundational for lots of future
applications of PCT in this area.

Please send me some examples of your work if there are things you’d like to share. I’m definitely interested.

And thanks to all for the birthday greetings! I’ve had a fun birthday going with two of my young grandsons to the chocolate museum in Hamburg, Germany.

Kent

···

On Sun, Sep 24, 2017 at 5:48 AM, Angus Jenkinson
angus@angusjenkinson.com wrote:

[Angus Jenkinson, 2017-09-23.11.46]

Kent, Bruce Nevin has suggested that I should reach out to you in relationship to some work on modelling that you have been doing on collective behaviour. I am interested, and
working with, the behaviour of organisations and social groups and the relationship between the individual and the group. I understand you have done some computer modelling of this. Is there something you can share?

And happy birthday!

………………………… ¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦……………………….

Angus Jenkinson

Regards,

Christopher Paul Geddes

Email: geddes.chris@gmail.com

Mobile: +1-876-393-1366

Hi Kent,

I saw it with delay. Happy birthday and happy future life with as much as possible minimum »errors«.

Boris

···

From: McClelland, Kent [mailto:MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU]
Sent: Sunday, September 24, 2017 9:20 PM
To: csgnet@lists.illinois.edu
Subject: Re: Collective Behaviour

[from Kent McClelland 2017.09.24.1735]

Hi Angus (and Christopher),

I’ve done a lot of thinking and some PCT-based modeling that relates to questions of collective action, organizations, and, more broadly, how society is possible.

The modeling I’ve done may strike you as pretty simplistic for your purposes, but the best summary of my work along that line can be found in a paper published in 2014 in the journal Sociological Theory called “Cycles of Conflict: A Computational Alternative to Collins’s Theory of Conflict Escalation.” You can download a copy of this paper, as well as my other published papers on PCT, from my page in ResearchGate.

A much longer and more detailed, but strictly theoretical, exposition of my ideas about “collective control”, as I’ve called it, is contained in my chapter for the Living Control Systems IV volume being published by Bill Powers’s sister, Alice. The chapter is called “Social Structure and Control: Perceptual Control Theory and the Science of Sociology.”

Unfortunately, the LCS IV volume has been slow to appear and still remains unpublished. I don’t know when it will be out, but the publisher has requested that authors not distribute any draft copies of the chapters prior to publication, so I can’t share my chapter with you at this point. I’ve posted a summary of the chapter on ResearchGate, but it’s probably too condensed to provide any very good sense of my argument.

Martin Taylor’s contribution to the LCS IV volume may also be of interest to you when that book appears. I’ve seen some early drafts of his chapters, and many of his ideas were immediately influential to me as I put my own chapter together. He comes at the question of how to apply PCT to social interaction from a somewhat different angle than I do, but his work is unlike anything else I’ve seen, and I think it will prove foundational for lots of future applications of PCT in this area.

Please send me some examples of your work if there are things you’d like to share. I’m definitely interested.

And thanks to all for the birthday greetings! I’ve had a fun birthday going with two of my young grandsons to the chocolate museum in Hamburg, Germany.

Kent

On Sep 24, 2017, at 3:08 PM, Christopher Paul Geddes geddes.chris@gmail.com wrote:

Angus,

Thanks for writing. I hope all is well. I have done some modeling in the past however I was kind of limited and now I am more focused on Systems Dynamics which is making me take more of an interest in modeling now as well.

I would like to know more about what you are looking for if I can help. I like to think of organizations as entities that I can influence with technology so that things so the way the team wants them to go.

Christopher

On Sun, Sep 24, 2017 at 5:48 AM, Angus Jenkinson angus@angusjenkinson.com wrote:

[Angus Jenkinson, 2017-09-23.11.46]

Kent, Bruce Nevin has suggested that I should reach out to you in relationship to some work on modelling that you have been doing on collective behaviour. I am interested, and working with, the behaviour of organisations and social groups and the relationship between the individual and the group. I understand you have done some computer modelling of this. Is there something you can share?

And happy birthday!

………â€â€¦â€¦……………………………………………………….………….

Angus Jenkinson

Regards,

Christopher Paul Geddes

Email: geddes.chris@gmail.com

Mobile: +1-876-393-1366

[Angus Jenkinson (2017-09-26. 23.57)] Good night

Thank you Kent, I am very grateful to you for sharing this.

I have started studying your paper and I can see the elegance of having a mathematical model for the purposes. But also analytical thinking. I regard it ass a major contribution. More of which later…

Although I have worked with conflict, it is not a speciality of mine. So what follows is an enquiry.

  1. You say: “Of course, every approach to modeling relies on simplifications and abstractions, and degrees of realism exhibited by agent-based models will vary with the level of detail built into the modeled agents…â€? You also say that it does not reproduce the detaiil of Collins’ explanation. Understood, but one of the cybernetic principles was that the best model was reality itself. Moreover, you comment that Collins does not give precise enough definitions. But part of the problem of “definitionâ€? is that it tends to reduce the variety of the phenomenon to a boundary defined by the definition. So I am interested in the degree to which you think that real life (sic) can be reproduced in a mathematical model? I return to this below…

<

  1. Building on that, as an illustration, you say:

  2. The other core variables in Collins’s model also lack of the kind of conceptual precision needed to support mathematical reformulation. Collins explicitly defines the variable atrocities as a matter of the perceptions of people on one side of the conflict interaction: “Atrocities are opponents’ actions that we perceive as especially hurtful and evil . . .� (2012:2). However, he does not offer any operational definition that would enable observers to judge whether a particular action committed by one side is likely to be defined as an atrocity by the other. My emphasis

  3. One of the conditions of conflict I understand is that the supposedly rational judgement is lost and people tend to find unacceptable, and perhaps extremely so, based on, let’s call it, a biased reaction. So what the person deems to be an atrocity might not be seen as such by an observer. That’s easily understood.

  4. One of the rediscoveries of neuroscience is the role of emotion in decision making. I am interested in how the strength and roles of those appear in the model?

  5. You refer to reference values as memories (aka mental pictures). So when you say you “will take polarization to refer to the extent of differences in reference values�, I take that to mean that the (actively filtered and probably distorted) perception is compared by each party to something conjured from their own memories (forgive the unmathematical description).

  6. You also say, “PCT models show the output of the two interacting systems diverging, with one system, in effect, pulling in one direction to bring the environmental variable into line with its preferences, while the other system pulls in the other (see McClelland 2004, 2006). Hence, the variable used in my simulations is the intensity of the conflict, measured by the degree of divergence in system outputs.�Outputs are to the control variable of the other as I understand it.

  7. So how do you process emotion in the model? Â

  8. How do you see the model contributing to practitioner work? And what other variables would need to be included? Is it more than offering a conceptual framework to think differently? Meaning, how would a version be used in Trump v Kim JU?

  9. It seems that being able to prove similar results to positive feedbacks through negative feedback is very useful in rethinking what happens.

  10. It also seems to me it replicates what practitioners in conflict resolution do. They ask each party to see how the other party sees things. Negotiators teach the same thing.

  11. Might however the +ve and –ve be mirrors of each other according to how one analysiis the situation? One is dealing with a series of escalating outcomes that propel each other and the other a series of negative (internal) cancellations that also effectively propel each other? Your strength is to show the interaction, maybe?

  12. The paper also introduces some clarity in thinking about issues. This is a typical case in all model building and as long as one does not allow a spurious impression of detail to get in the way, can help. I like for example the parsimonious description: “From the PCT perspective, conflict occurs whenever the two sides use different reference standards in their attempts to control an environmental variable, and victory or defeat in the contest is represented by the extent to which side or the other succeeds in bringing the variable into line with its own preferred reference conditions�.

  13. I wonder whether it does not verbally – as a result of your thinking through the issues with the help of modelling – give the practitioner as good an insight as a mathematicall model?

BTW: You mention: ““reflex arc� as conventionally understood�; which version of this do you mean? Presumably not the one that Dewey critiqued?

—&nbspp;

A

[from Kent McClelland 2017.09.24.1735]

Hi Angus (and Christopher),

I’ve done a lot of thinking and some PCT-based modeling that relates to questions of collective action, organizations, and, more broadly, how society is possible.

The modeling I’ve done may strike you as pretty simplistic for your purposes, but the best summary of my work along that line can be found in a paper published in 2014 in the journal Sociological Theory called “Cycles of Conflict: A Computational Alternative to Collins’s Theory of Conflict Escalation.” You can download a copy of this paper, as well as my other published papers on PCT, from my page in ResearchGate.

A much longer and more detailed, but strictly theoretical, exposition of my ideas about “collective control”, as I’ve called it, is contained in my chapter for the Living Control Systems IV volume being published by Bill Powers’s sister, Alice. The chapter is called “Social Structure and Control: Perceptual Control Theory and the Science of Sociology.”

Unfortunately, the LCS IV volume has been slow to appear and still remains unpublished. I don’t know when it will be out, but the publisher has requested that authors not distribute any draft copies of the chapters prior to publication, so I can’t share my chapter with you at this point. I’ve posted a summary of the chapter on ResearchGate, but it’s probably too condensed to provide any very good sense of my argument.

Martin Taylor’s contribution to the LCS IV volume may also be of interest to you when that book appears. I’ve seen some early drafts of his chapters, and many of his ideas were immediately influential to me as I put my own chapter together. He comes at the question of how to apply PCT to social interaction from a somewhat different angle than I do, but his work is unlike anything else I’ve seen, and I think it will prove foundational for lots of future applications of PCT in this area.

Please send me some examples of your work if there are things you’d like to share. I’m definitely interested.

And thanks to all for the birthday greetings! I’ve had a fun birthday going with two of my young grandsons to the chocolate museum in Hamburg, Germany.

Kent

···

On 24/09/2017, 20:20, “McClelland, Kent” MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU wrote:

On Sep 24, 2017, at 3:08 PM, Christopher Paul Geddes geddes.chris@gmail.com wrote:

Angus,

Thanks for writing. I hope all is well. I have done some modeling in the past however I was kind of limited and now I am more focused on Systems Dynamics which is making me take more of an interest in modeling now as well.

I would like to know more about what you are looking for if I can help. I like to think of organizations as entities that I can influence with technology so that things so the way the team wants them to go.

Christopher

On Sun, Sep 24, 2017 at 5:48 AM, Angus Jenkinson angus@angusjenkinson.com wrote:

[Angus Jenkinson, 2017-09-23.11.46]

Kent, Bruce Nevin has suggested that I should reach out to you in relationship to some work on modelling that you have been doing on collective behaviour. I am interested, and working with, the behaviour of organisations and social groups and the relationship between the individual and the group. I understand you have done some computer modelling of this. Is there something you can share?

And happy birthday!

………â¦â€¦â€¦………………… ¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦………………………………….

Angus Jenkinson

Regards,

Christopher Paul Geddes

Email: geddes.chris@gmail.com

Mobile: +1-876-393-1366

[Angus Jenkinson: 2017-09-26.12.08]

Kent,

Following up further on your interesting paper: it seems to me that the model that you create for the analysis of conflict could also be adapted for the analysis of the design of change, a field with which I work. It would need some more parameters added, but essentially it concerns the question of how agent 1 could act in such a way as to change the behaviours of patients 2-n by (essentially) marker changes in the context field and therefore situation of those agents, terms that can be defined.

That might be interesting to work on?

Angus

[from Kent McClelland 2017.09.24.1735]

Hi Angus (and Christopher),

I’ve done a lot of thinking and some PCT-based modeling that relates to questions of collective action, organizations, and, more broadly, how society is possible.

The modeling I’ve done may strike you as pretty simplistic for your purposes, but the best summary of my work along that line can be found in a paper published in 2014 in the journal Sociological Theory called “Cycles of Conflict: A Computational Alternative to Collins’s Theory of Conflict Escalation.” You can download a copy of this paper, as well as my other published papers on PCT, from my page in ResearchGate.

A much longer and more detailed, but strictly theoretical, exposition of my ideas about “collective control”, as I’ve called it, is contained in my chapter for the Living Control Systems IV volume being published by Bill Powers’s sister, Alice. The chapter is called “Social Structure and Control: Perceptual Control Theory and the Science of Sociology.”

Unfortunately, the LCS IV volume has been slow to appear and still remains unpublished. I don’t know when it will be out, but the publisher has requested that authors not distribute any draft copies of the chapters prior to publication, so I can’t share my chapter with you at this point. I’ve posted a summary of the chapter on ResearchGate, but it’s probably too condensed to provide any very good sense of my argument.

Martin Taylor’s contribution to the LCS IV volume may also be of interest to you when that book appears. I’ve seen some early drafts of his chapters, and many of his ideas were immediately influential to me as I put my own chapter together. He comes at the question of how to apply PCT to social interaction from a somewhat different angle than I do, but his work is unlike anything else I’ve seen, and I think it will prove foundational for lots of future applications of PCT in this area.

Please send me some examples of your work if there are things you’d like to share. I’m definitely interested.

And thanks to all for the birthday greetings! I’ve had a fun birthday going with two of my young grandsons to the chocolate museum in Hamburg, Germany.

Kent

···

On 24/09/2017, 20:20, “McClelland, Kent” MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU wrote:

On Sep 24, 2017, at 3:08 PM, Christopher Paul Geddes geddes.chris@gmail.com wrote:

Angus,

Thanks for writing. I hope all is well. I have done some modeling in the past however I was kind of limited and now I am more focused on Systems Dynamics which is making me take more of an interest in modeling now as well.

I would like to know more about what you are looking for if I can help. I like to think of organizations as entities that I can influence with technology so that things so the way the team wants them to go.

Christopher

On Sun, Sep 24, 2017 at 5:48 AM, Angus Jenkinson angus@angusjenkinson.com wrote:

[Angus Jenkinson, 2017-09-23.11.46]

Kent, Bruce Nevin has suggested that I should reach out to you in relationship to some work on modelling that you have been doing on collective behaviour. I am interested, and working with, the behaviour of organisations and social groups and the relationship between the individual and the group. I understand you have done some computer modelling of this. Is there something you can share?

And happy birthday!

…………………¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦……………………………………………….

Angus Jenkinson

Regards,

Christopher Paul Geddes

Email: geddes.chris@gmail.com

Mobile: +1-876-393-1366

[Angus Jenkinson: 2017-09-26.12.08]

Kent,

Following up further on your interesting paper: it seems to me that the model that you create for the analysis of conflict could also be adapted for the
analysis of the design of change, a field with which I work. It would need some more parameters added, but essentially it concerns the question of how agent 1 could act in such a way as to change the behaviours of patients 2-n by (essentially) marker changes
in the context field and therefore situation of those agents, terms that can be defined.

That might be interesting to work on?

Angus

···

On 24/09/2017, 20:20, “McClelland, Kent” MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU wrote:

[from Kent McClelland 2017.09.24.1735]

Hi Angus (and Christopher),

I’ve done a lot of thinking and some PCT-based modeling that relates to questions of collective action, organizations, and, more broadly, how society is possible.

The modeling I’ve done may strike you as pretty simplistic for your purposes, but the best summary of my work along that line can be found in a paper published in 2014 in the journal Sociological Theory called “Cycles of Conflict: A Computational
Alternative to Collins’s Theory of Conflict Escalation.” You can download a copy of this paper, as well as my other published papers on PCT, from my page in ResearchGate.

A much longer and more detailed, but strictly theoretical, exposition of my ideas about “collective control”, as I’ve called it, is contained in my chapter for the Living Control Systems IV volume being published by Bill Powers’s sister,
Alice. The chapter is called “Social Structure and Control: Perceptual Control Theory and the Science of Sociology.”

Unfortunately, the LCS IV volume has been slow to appear and still remains unpublished. I don’t know when it will be out, but the publisher has requested that authors not distribute any draft copies of the chapters prior to publication,
so I can’t share my chapter with you at this point. I’ve posted a summary of the chapter on ResearchGate, but it’s probably too condensed to provide any very good sense of my argument.

Martin Taylor’s contribution to the LCS IV volume may also be of interest to you when that book appears. I’ve seen some early drafts of his chapters, and many of his ideas were immediately influential to me as I put my own chapter together.
He comes at the question of how to apply PCT to social interaction from a somewhat different angle than I do, but his work is unlike anything else I’ve seen, and I think it will prove foundational for lots of future applications of PCT in this area.

Please send me some examples of your work if there are things you’d like to share. I’m definitely interested.

And thanks to all for the birthday greetings! I’ve had a fun birthday going with two of my young grandsons to the chocolate museum in Hamburg, Germany.

Kent

On Sep 24, 2017, at 3:08 PM, Christopher Paul Geddes geddes.chris@gmail.com wrote:

Angus,

Thanks for writing. I hope all is well. I have done some modeling in the past however I was kind of limited and now I am more focused on Systems Dynamics which is making me take more of an interest in modeling now as well.

I would like to know more about what you are looking for if I can help. I like to think of organizations as entities that I can influence with technology so that things so the way the team wants them to go.

Christopher

On Sun, Sep 24, 2017 at 5:48 AM, Angus Jenkinson angus@angusjenkinson.com wrote:

[Angus Jenkinson, 2017-09-23.11.46]

Kent, Bruce Nevin has suggested that I should reach out to you in relationship to some work on modelling that you have
been doing on collective behaviour. I am interested, and working with, the behaviour of organisations and social groups and the relationship between the individual and the group. I understand you have done some computer modelling of this. Is there something
you can share?

And happy birthday!

…………………………â………………………………………….

Angus Jenkinson

Regards,

Christopher Paul Geddes

Email: geddes.chris@gmail.com

Mobile: +1-876-393-1366

[Angus
Jenkinson (2017-09-26. 23.57)] Good night

Â

        Thank

you Kent, I am very grateful to you for sharing this.

Â

        I

have started studying your paper and I can see the elegance
of having a mathematical model for the purposes. But also
analytical thinking. I regard it ass a major contribution.Â
More of which later…

Â

        Although

I have worked with conflict, it is not a speciality of mine.
So what follows is an enquiry.

Â

···
  1.           One
    

of the rediscoveries of neuroscience is the role of
emotion in decision making. I am interested in how the
strength and roles of those appear in the model?

  1.             You
    

refer to reference values as memories (aka mental
pictures).

  1.             You
    

also say, “PCT models show the output of the two
interacting systems diverging, with one system, in
effect, pulling in one direction to bring the
environmental variable into line with its preferences,
while the other system pulls in the other (see
McClelland 2004, 2006). Hence, the variable used in my
simulations is the intensity of the conflict, measured
by the degree of divergence in system outputs.�Outputs
are to the control variable of the other as I understand
it.

  1.           How
    

do you see the model contributing to practitioner work?
And what other variables would need to be included? Is it
more than offering a conceptual framework to think
differently? Meaning, how would a version be used in Trump
v Kim JU?

  1.             It
    

seems that being able to prove similar results to
positive feedbacks through negative feedback is very
useful in rethinking what happens.

  1.             It
    

also seems to me it replicates what practitioners in
conflict resolution do. They ask each party to see how
the other party sees things. Negotiators teach the same
thing.

  1.             Might
    

however the +ve and –ve be mirrors of each other
according to how one analysis the situation? One is
dealing with a series of escalating outcomes that propel
each other and the other a series of negative (internal)
cancellations that also effectively propel each other?
Your strength is to show the interaction, maybe?

  1.             The
    

paper also introduces some clarity in thinking about
issues. This is a typical case in all model building and
as long as one does not allow a spurious impression of
detail to get in the way, can help. I like for example
the parsimonious description: “From the PCT perspective,
conflict occurs whenever the two sides use different
reference standards in their attempts to control an
environmental variable, and victory or defeat in the
contest is represented by the extent to which side or
the other succeeds in bringing the variable into line
with its own preferred reference conditions�.

  1.             I
    

wonder whether it does not verbally – as a result of
your thinking through the issues with the help of
modelling – give the practitioner as good an insighht as
a mathematical model?

Â

        BTW:

You mention: ““reflex arc� as conventionally understood�;
which version of this do you mean? Presumably not the one
that Dewey critiqued?

Â

—Â

A

Â

Â

          On 24/09/2017, 20:20, "McClelland,

Kent" <MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU >
wrote:

Â

[from Kent McClelland 2017.09.24.1735]

Â

Hi Angus (and Christopher),

Â

        I've done a lot of thinking and some

PCT-based modeling that relates to questions of collective
action, organizations, and, more broadly, how society is
possible.Â

Â

        The modeling I've done may strike you as

pretty simplistic for your purposes, but the best summary of
my work along that line can be found in a paper published in
2014 in the journal Sociological Theory called “Cycles of
Conflict: A Computational Alternative to Collins’s Theory of
Conflict Escalation.” You can download a copy of this paper,
as well as my other published papers on PCT, from my page in
ResearchGate.

Â

        A much longer and more detailed, but

strictly theoretical, exposition of my ideas about
“collective control”, as I’ve called it, is contained in my
chapter for the Living Control Systems IV volume being
published by Bill Powers’s sister, Alice. The chapter is
called "Social Structure and Control: Perceptual Control
Theory and the Science of Sociology."Â

Â

        Unfortunately, the LCS IV volume has been

slow to appear and still remains unpublished. I don’t know
when it will be out, but the publisher has requested that
authors not distribute any draft copies of the chapters
prior to publication, so I can’t share my chapter with you
at this point. I’ve posted a summary of the chapter
on ResearchGate, but it’s probably too condensed to provide
any very good sense of my argument.

        Martin Taylor's contribution to the LCS

IV volume may also be of interest to you when that book
appears. I’ve seen some early drafts of his chapters, and
many of his ideas were immediately influential to me as I
put my own chapter together. He comes at the question of how
to apply PCT to social interaction from a somewhat different
angle than I do, but his work is unlike anything else I’ve
seen, and I think it will prove foundational for lots of
future applications of PCT in this area.Â

        Please send me some examples of your work

if there are things you’d like to share. I’m definitely
interested.Â

        And thanks to all for the birthday

greetings! I’ve had a fun birthday going with two of my
young grandsons to the chocolate museum in Hamburg, Germany.

KentÂ

        On Sep 24, 2017, at 3:08 PM, Christopher Paul Geddes <geddes.chris@gmail.com            >

wrote:

Angus,

Â

              Thanks for writing. I hope all is

well. I have done some modeling in the past however I
was kind of limited and now I am more focused on
Systems Dynamics which is making me take more of an
interest in modeling now as well.Â

Â

              I would like to know more about

what you are looking for if I can help. I like to
think of organizations as entities that I can
influence with technology so that things so the way
the team wants them to go.

Â

Christopher

Â

              On Sun, Sep 24, 2017 at 5:48 AM,

Angus Jenkinson <angus@angusjenkinson.com >
wrote:

                      [Angus

Jenkinson, 2017-09-23.11.46]

Â

                      Kent,

Bruce Nevin has suggested that I should reach
out to you in relationship to some work on
modelling that you have been doing on
collective behaviour. I am interested, and
working with, the behaviour of organisations
and social groups and the relationship between
the individual and the group. I understand
you have done some computer modelling of
this. Is there something you can share?

Â

                      And happy

birthday!

Â

………â¦â€¦â€¦………………… ¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦………………………………….

Angus Jenkinson

Â

Regards,

Â

                              Christopher Paul

Geddes

Â

Email:Â geddes.chris@gmail.com

                              Mobile:

+1-876-393-1366

Â

Â

Angus Jenkinson (2017-10-04. 17:14)

Martin

Thank you for the reference.

See below

···

………………â€â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦………………………………….

Angus Jenkinson

On 07/10/2017, 15:05, “Martin Taylor” mmt-csg@mmtaylor.net wrote:

[Martin Taylor 2017.09.27.10.40]

I won’t even try to speak for the departing Kent, but I do have a comment or two…

[Angus Jenkinson (2017-09-26. 23.57)] Good night

Thank you Kent, I am very grateful to you for sharing this.

I have started studying your paper and I can see the elegance of having a mathematical model for the purposes. But also analytical
thinking. I regard it ass a major contribution. More of which later…

Although I have worked with conflict, it is not a speciality of mine. So what follows is an enquiry.

One of the rediscoveries of neuroscience is the role of emotion in decision making. I am interested in how the strength and roles of those appear in
the model?

You could check out the “Emotion” chapter in the second edition of B:CP. You may or may not agree with much of it, but it is something Bill Powers wrote on the question. It seems to me, however, that it deals only with unwanted emotions such as grief, fear,
sadness, and not with gladness, pleasure, attraction and other desirable emotions.

Is not “emotion” such as “sadness” or “fear” a phenomenon of qualia, such as redness or sweetness? PCT does not address the nature of qualia as such, but it does deal with the functional relationship of variables such as the amount of redness, which might influence
a decision as to whether to buy a particular chair. In that vein, I would ask whether the amount of emotion X is a functional component of a decision-making process or a side-effect that occurs as a consequence of the process. I would not expect it to appear
directly in a PCT model unless as a perceptual variable that had a magnitude that either was controlled (“I don’t want to feel so sad”) or that was a component of a variable that was controlled.

To call emotion qualia means to use an abstraction that is a derived from a particular epistemology. It is also have a quite different order from sweetness. Emotion is for example a response to sweetness. One of them is a concept of
a sensory/sensual experience and the other is an emotional response to it. Both of these sit within of course the individual’s hierarchy. I think your general discussion here goes some way towards explaining why ternary cybernetics is required. This adds
the domain of imparity to those of information and energy.

a.
You refer to reference values as memories (aka mental pictures).

Powers proposed that as a possibility, but as I understand the implications of his model, many (all?) reference values are composites built from the outputs of higher-level control units. When he references “memories”, they are the outputs of associative memory
units (in the computer sense) addressed by the vector of values of these higher-level outputs. Such memories may not be exact, but may be composites of earlier perceptions.

A question raised by Powers was what memories would be stored and when, since it would clearly be impossible to store all detail at millisecond time resolution of what was in the perceptual field at any one moment. So what triggers a storage event? I suggest
that a perception might be stored as a memory and continuously amended so long as it is being changed by control, but ceases changing and becomes more stabilized when the perception ceases changing because it is near its reference value. That is rather as
reorganization works. There’s no decision “here we are, now stop”, but changes become smaller and less frequent, stabilizing near useful values.

As I said, I can’t speak for Kent on that or anything else on which you commented, but I can offer my unwanted opinion on one or two other things you say.

Thank you. A Gestalt, which would be a useful starting point for thinking about a mental picture, includes an immense amount of detail, which might therefore be called a composite. It is certainly a composition. But it is concrete.
But I can compare the carpet with my memory of the carpet. One is my current perception and the other is my mental picture deriving from memory. The comparison is a function of intellect, as intellect is normally understood. The perception of difference
might lead me to certain behaviour, such as cleaning the carpet or deciding it is not worthwhile. I think that description can map onto what you are describing.

b.

c.
You also say, “PCT models show the output of the two interacting systems diverging, with one system, in effect, pulling in one direction to bring the
environmental variable into line with its preferences, while the other system pulls in the other (see McClelland 2004, 2006). Hence, the variable used in my simulations is the intensity of the conflict, measured by the degree of divergence in system outputs.�Outputs
are to the control variable of the other as I understand it.

Not in my understanding. Outputs are to the external environment. In that environment an output from control by A of some perception X may (and in the case of conflict do) influence something B perceives, in such a way as to move B’s perceptual value away from
its reference value. The resulting action by B similarly acts on something (not necessarily the same thing) in the environment so as to move A’s perception of it away from its reference value. If B’s action influences A’s perception of X, it completes a positive
feedback loop, which manifests as conflict. If B’s action influences A’s perception of Y rather than X, and A’s control of Y alters A’s reference value for X, that also could lead to a positive feedback loop, and escalating conflict.

The positive feedback loop is the mechanism of conflict, however it may be established, but not all positive feedback loops indicate conflict, just as not all negative feedback loops are control loops.

I leave the rest to Kent.

Martin

Thank you for clarifying

  1. How do you see the model contributing to practitioner work? And what other variables would need to be
    included? Is it more than offering a conceptual framework to think differently? Meaning, how would a version be used in Trump v Kim JU?
  1. It seems that being able to prove similar results to positive feedbacks through negative feedback is
    very useful in rethinking what happens.
  1. It also seems to me it replicates what practitioners in conflict resolution do. They ask each party to
    see how the other party sees things. Negotiators teach the same thing.
  1. Might however the +ve and –ve be mirrors of each otheer according to how one analysis the situation? One
    is dealing with a series of escalating outcomes that propel each other and the other a series of negative (internal) cancellations that also effectively propel each other? Your strength is to show the interaction, maybe?
  1. The paper also introduces some clarity in thinking about issues. This is a typical case in all model
    building and as long as one does not allow a spurious impression of detail to get in the way, can help. I like for example the parsimonious description: “From the PCT perspective, conflict occurs whenever the two sides use different reference standards in
    their attempts to control an environmental variable, and victory or defeat in the contest is represented by the extent to which side or the other succeeds in bringing the variable into line with its own preferred reference conditions�.
  1. I wonder whether it does not verbally – as a ressult of your thinking through the issues with the help
    of modelling – give the practitiioner as good an insight as a mathematical model?

BTW: You mention: ““reflex arc� as conventionally understood�; which version of this do you mean? Presumably not the one that Dewey critiqued?

— ;

A

On 24/09/2017, 20:20, “McClelland, Kent” MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU wrote:

[from Kent McClelland 2017.09.24.1735]

Hi Angus (and Christopher),

I’ve done a lot of thinking and some PCT-based modeling that relates to questions of collective action, organizations, and, more broadly, how society is possible.

The modeling I’ve done may strike you as pretty simplistic for your purposes, but the best summary of my work along that line can be found in a paper published in 2014 in the journal Sociological Theory called “Cycles of Conflict: A Computational
Alternative to Collins’s Theory of Conflict Escalation.” You can download a copy of this paper, as well as my other published papers on PCT, from my page in ResearchGate.

A much longer and more detailed, but strictly theoretical, exposition of my ideas about “collective control”, as I’ve called it, is contained in my chapter for the Living Control Systems IV volume being published by Bill Powers’s sister,
Alice. The chapter is called “Social Structure and Control: Perceptual Control Theory and the Science of Sociology.”

Unfortunately, the LCS IV volume has been slow to appear and still remains unpublished. I don’t know when it will be out, but the publisher has requested that authors not distribute any draft copies of the chapters prior to publication,
so I can’t share my chapter with you at this point. I’ve posted a summary of the chapter on ResearchGate, but it’s probably too condensed to provide any very good sense of my argument.

Martin Taylor’s contribution to the LCS IV volume may also be of interest to you when that book appears. I’ve seen some early drafts of his chapters, and many of his ideas were immediately influential to me as I put my own chapter together.
He comes at the question of how to apply PCT to social interaction from a somewhat different angle than I do, but his work is unlike anything else I’ve seen, and I think it will prove foundational for lots of future applications of PCT in this area.

Please send me some examples of your work if there are things you’d like to share. I’m definitely interested.

And thanks to all for the birthday greetings! I’ve had a fun birthday going with two of my young grandsons to the chocolate museum in Hamburg, Germany.

Kent

On Sep 24, 2017, at 3:08 PM, Christopher Paul Geddes geddes.chris@gmail.com wrote:

Angus,

Thanks for writing. I hope all is well. I have done some modeling in the past however I was kind of limited and now I am more focused on Systems Dynamics which is making me take more of an interest in modeling now as well.

I would like to know more about what you are looking for if I can help. I like to think of organizations as entities that I can influence with technology so that things so the way the team wants them to go.

Christopher

On Sun, Sep 24, 2017 at 5:48 AM, Angus Jenkinson angus@angusjenkinson.com wrote:

[Angus Jenkinson, 2017-09-23.11.46]

Kent, Bruce Nevin has suggested that I should reach out to you in relationship to some work on modelling that you have
been doing on collective behaviour. I am interested, and working with, the behaviour of organisations and social groups and the relationship between the individual and the group. I understand you have done some computer modelling of this. Is there something
you can share?

And happy birthday!

……………………………………………………………………….

Angus Jenkinson

Regards,

Christopher Paul Geddes

Email: geddes.chris@gmail.com

Mobile: +1-876-393-1366

from Kent McClelland (2017.10.09.1500 EDT)
Re: Angus Jenkinson (2017-09-26. 23.57), Martin Taylor 2017.09.27.10.40
HI Angus and Martin,
At long last, I’m back in Grinnell and have had some time to think about the questions you asked, Angus. I’ll start with a response to that post and treat Martin’s comments in another.

AJ: Thank you Kent, I am very grateful to you for sharing this.

I have started studying your paper and I can see the elegance of having a mathematical model for the purposes. But also analytical thinking. I regard it ass a major contribution. More of which later…

KM: Thanks!

AJ: Although I have worked with conflict, it is not a speciality of mine. So what follows is an enquiry.

KM: My focus on conflict has led me to see it as a ubiquitous feature of social interaction, just as cooperation. collaboration, or coordination of one kind or another occur in almost every interaction. I think of conflict and cooperation, then, as two varieties of the more general phenomenon of collective control. So, I’d say that anytime you study social interaction you’re implicitly dealing with conflict, and cooperation, too, of course.
KM: Granted that most everyday interactions between people don’t take the form of a conflict escalation spiral, like those described by Collins and analyzed in my paper. The reason that most conflicts don’t get out of hand, as I see it, is that everyday interactions nearly always require collective control at a variety of perceptual levels simultaneously, and while conflict may be occurring at one perceptual level, cooperation more typically occurs other levels of control. Thus, for us to do a comprehensive analysis of any given social interaction, we have to think in terms of the participants' collective control of perceptions on a hierarchy from low-level perceptions of the shared physical environment to the middle-level perceptions involved in language and communication, to high-level perceptions, such as norms or social identities.
KM: Some examples: Two sports teams are in conflict over which team will win the game, but they cooperate at low levels of perceptual control by playing together on the same field or pitch, using the same ball, etc., and at higher levels of control by adhering to the perceptual principles defining the rules of the game. In most contests, the conflict between the teams doesn’t ever escalate into unbridled conflict. However, sometimes a member of one team commits what appears to be a deliberate foul that injures a player on the other team (or just humiliates him). A series of tit-for-tat physical aggressions may then occur as the teams seek to even the moral score, and we see an example of the familiar conflict-escalation pattern.
KM: Two people argue with each other. Cooperative low-level collective control nevertheless occurs as they use a common language and orient their bodies to face each other (although at these low perceptual levels some of their physical gestures may symbolize threats of violence to the other). While everyday arguments commonly result in patterns of conflict escalation, as the participants trade increasingly cutting insults, for instance, most arguments don’t degenerate into fisticuffs. A typical reason for this restraint may be that the participants are cooperatively controlling some higher-level perception, perhaps a mutual friendship or relationship that they want to preserve. Another possibility is that both of them recognize and adhere to higher-level perceptions of norms that prohibit doing violence to another person or prescribe penalties for physical aggression.
KM: In general, the conflict-resolution literature suggests that one good way to resolve a conflict is to focus the combatants' attention on some goal that they both share, or in other words to get them to cooperatively control some higher-level perception, rather than conflictively controlling the low-level perceptions that they are fighting about. Interestingly, the MOL method of therapy operates on a similar principle of directing the attention of a client caught in an internal conflict to some higher-level perception connected to the two lower-level perceptions in conflict, which then enables the client to prioritize which of the incompatible lower-level perceptions to control, or do something else entirely.
KM: Another example: A work team or a committee work together to complete a project or reach some other common goal. The overall tenor of the interaction is cooperative, but along the way lots of little conflicts pop up about the best means toward reaching their goals. If the group is working together effectively, these conflicts are handled by talking these conflicts out with each other or else deferring to a boss whose word is taken as authoritative and who tells them what to do. The basic purpose of verbal communication, in my view, is to enable people to align the reference values to be used for their collective control of perceptions. What communication communicates, I would say, are reference values for the perceptions that the speaker wants the listener to control. People who want to work together cooperatively need well aligned reference values for their common perceptions, and lack of alignment is tantamount to conflict.

1. AJ: You say: “Of course, every approach to modeling relies on simplifications and abstractions, and degrees of realism exhibited by agent-based models will vary with the level of detail built into the modeled agents…â€? You also say that it dooes not reproduce the detail of Collins’ explanation. Understood, but one of the cybernetic principles was that the best model was reality itself. Moreover, you comment that Collins does not give precise enough definitions. But part of the problem of “definitionâ€? is that it tends to reduce the variety of the phenomenon to a boundary defined by the definition. So I am interested in the degree to which you think that real life (sic) can be reproduced in a mathematical model? I return to this below…

KM: The highly simplified mathematical models that I used in my paper pertain to only a single level of perception. When multiple levels of perception are in play, as in the examples I’ve suggested above, these simple mathematical models obviously can’t begin to reproduce the real-life complexity of cooperation on one level of perception and conflict on another. More sophisticated multi-level mathematical models might be able to do a little better job, but it would take an enormously complex mathematical model to simulate the multi-level complexity of the real-life interaction. What mathematical models are good for, in my view, is to alert us to the basic patterns of dynamic interaction we might expect to see in these real-life instances and also to provide a theoretically grounded explanation of why such patterns are likely to emerge.

2. Building on that, as an illustration, you say:

  1. The other core variables in Collins’s model also lack of the kind of conceptual precision needed to support mathematical reformulation. Collins explicitly defines the variable atrocities as a matter of the perceptions of people on one side of the conflict interaction: “Atrocities are opponents’ actions that we perceive as especially hurtful and evil . . .â€? (2012:2). However, he does not offer any operational definition that would enable observers to judge whether a particular action committed by one side is likely to be defined as an atrocity by the other. My emphasis
  2. One of the conditions of conflict I understand is that the supposedly rational judgement is lost and people tend to find unacceptable, and perhaps extremely so, based on, let’s call it, a biased reaction. So what the person deems to be an atrocity might not be seen as such by an observer. That’s easily understood.

KM: Nevertheless, many of the violent atrocities that Collins describes would strike almost any observer as atrocities. When you kill scores of people, for example, that’s an atrocity in anybody’s book. My definition of violence, from a PCT viewpoint, is the destruction of environmental feedback paths that someone else needs to keep their highly valued perceptions under control. When you kill someone, for instance, you render their physical body nonoperational, and the physical body always forms two essential links in the circuit of pathways used by individuals to control their perceptions: on the output side, the muscular movements that enable individuals to manipulate their physical environments, and on the input side, the sensory organs that report the effects of those manipulations. But violence can also consist of destroying physical objects that individuals rely on to control perceptions, like their houses or tools. It’s when you get to higher levels of perception, what Pierre Bourdieu referred to as symbolic violence, that definitions of what an atrocity might consist of get more subjective. Is destroying someone’s good name, for instance, an atrocity? It’s certainly an essential link in the feedback path that people use to maintain a positive sense of self.

1. One of the rediscoveries of neuroscience is the role of emotion in decision making. I am interested in how the strength and roles of those appear in the model?

  1. You refer to reference values as memories (aka mental pictures). So when you say you “will take polarization to refer to the extent of differences in reference valuesâ€?, I take that to mean that the (actively filtered and probably distorted) perception is compared by each party to something conjured from their own memories (forgive the unmathematical description).
  2. You also say, “PCT models show the output of the two interacting systems diverging, with one system, in effect, pulling in one direction to bring the environmental variable into line with its preferences, while the other system pulls in the other (see McClelland 2004, 2006). Hence, the variable used in my simulations is the intensity of the conflict, measured by the degree of divergence in system outputs.â€?Outputs are to the control variable of the other as I understand it.

KM: As Martin notes in his reply, one’s outputs affect the shared environmental reality, and the opponent controls a separate perception based on that environmental reality, but that perception isn’t necessarily based on the opponent’s perception of the individual’s output. What happens in conflict is that a positive feedback loop emerges that links the two opponents’ outputs, the portions of the environment on which both outputs are acting, and their respective perceptual control loops.

  2. So how do you process emotion in the model?

KM: Martin’s reply, directing you to Ch 17 in the second edition of Behavior: The Control of Perception, is a good start for answering the question about the place of emotion in this model. What I’d add is that Powers argues that the experience of prolonged inability to bring a valued perception under control inevitably produces negative emotions like frustration and anger. In conflict situations, the actions of the opponent by definition prevent one from bringing the contested perceptual variable into control at one’s own reference level, so the PCT definition of conflict implies that these situations always produce negative emotions. Emotion is just an expected part of conflict. The other thing I would say is that because people are operating on several different levels of perception at the same time, emotional reactions do not preclude rational behavior. The two processes can go on simultaneously.

1. How do you see the model contributing to practitioner work? And what other variables would need to be included? Is it more than offering a conceptual framework to think differently? Meaning, how would a version be used in Trump v Kim JU?

KM: The mud-slinging match between Trump and Kim sure looks like a classic conflict-escalation cycle to me. What my model does not do is predict the outcome of this escalating conflict, because it doesn’t take into account the other presumably higher-level perceptions that these two actors may be controlling. Let’s hope that there are some perceptions that restrain them before they start pushing nuclear buttons!

  1. It seems that being able to prove similar results to positive feedbacks through negative feedback is very useful in rethinking what happens.

  2. It also seems to me it replicates what practitioners in conflict resolution do. They ask each party to see how the other party sees things. Negotiators teach the same thing.

KM: Good point.

  2. Might however the +ve and –vee be mirrors of each other according to how one analysis the situation? One is dealing with a series of escalating outcomes that propel each other and the other a series of negative (internal) cancellations that also effectively propel each other? Your strength is to show the interaction, maybe?

KM: I’m not quite sure what you mean by the ±ve in the model. The value of the environmental variable [v] times what [e]?

  2. The paper also introduces some clarity in thinking about issues. This is a typical case in all model building and as long as one does not allow a spurious impression of detail to get in the way, can help. I like for example the parsimonious description: “From the PCT perspective, conflict occurs whenever the two sides use different reference standards in their attempts to control an environmental variable, and victory or defeat in the contest is represented by the extent to which side or the other succeeds in bringing the variable into line with its own preferred reference conditionsâ€?.

  3. I wonder whether it does not verbally – as a result of your thinking through the issues with the help of modelling – give the prractitioner as good an insight as a mathematical model?

KM: The model goes beyond the verbal description to show the dynamics of the interaction, something the verbal description can’t really do.

BTW: You mention: ““reflex arc� as conventionally understood�; which version of this do you mean? Presumably not the one that Dewey critiqued?

KM: No, I am talking about Dewey’s reflex arc, as well as the set of internal neural connections that provides the focus of most current neurological analyses.
KM: Thanks again for your good questions and your interest in my work!
Kent

···


A

On 24/09/2017, 20:20, "McClelland, Kent" <<mailto:MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU>MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU> wrote:

[from Kent McClelland 2017.09.24.1735]

Hi Angus (and Christopher),

I've done a lot of thinking and some PCT-based modeling that relates to questions of collective action, organizations, and, more broadly, how society is possible.

The modeling I've done may strike you as pretty simplistic for your purposes, but the best summary of my work along that line can be found in a paper published in 2014 in the journal Sociological Theory called "Cycles of Conflict: A Computational Alternative to Collins's Theory of Conflict Escalation." You can download a copy of this paper, as well as my other published papers on PCT, from my page in ResearchGate.

A much longer and more detailed, but strictly theoretical, exposition of my ideas about "collective control", as I've called it, is contained in my chapter for the Living Control Systems IV volume being published by Bill Powers's sister, Alice. The chapter is called "Social Structure and Control: Perceptual Control Theory and the Science of Sociology."

Unfortunately, the LCS IV volume has been slow to appear and still remains unpublished. I don't know when it will be out, but the publisher has requested that authors not distribute any draft copies of the chapters prior to publication, so I can't share my chapter with you at this point. I've posted a summary of the chapter on ResearchGate, but it's probably too condensed to provide any very good sense of my argument.

Martin Taylor's contribution to the LCS IV volume may also be of interest to you when that book appears. I've seen some early drafts of his chapters, and many of his ideas were immediately influential to me as I put my own chapter together. He comes at the question of how to apply PCT to social interaction from a somewhat different angle than I do, but his work is unlike anything else I've seen, and I think it will prove foundational for lots of future applications of PCT in this area.

Please send me some examples of your work if there are things you'd like to share. I'm definitely interested.

And thanks to all for the birthday greetings! I've had a fun birthday going with two of my young grandsons to the chocolate museum in Hamburg, Germany.

Kent
On Sep 24, 2017, at 3:08 PM, Christopher Paul Geddes <<mailto:geddes.chris@gmail.com>geddes.chris@gmail.com> wrote:

Angus,

Thanks for writing. I hope all is well. I have done some modeling in the past however I was kind of limited and now I am more focused on Systems Dynamics which is making me take more of an interest in modeling now as well.

I would like to know more about what you are looking for if I can help. I like to think of organizations as entities that I can influence with technology so that things so the way the team wants them to go.

Christopher

On Sun, Sep 24, 2017 at 5:48 AM, Angus Jenkinson <<mailto:angus@angusjenkinson.com>angus@angusjenkinson.com> wrote:

[Angus Jenkinson, 2017-09-23.11.46]

Kent, Bruce Nevin has suggested that I should reach out to you in relationship to some work on modelling that you have been doing on collective behaviour. I am interested, and working with, the behaviour of organisations and social groups and the relationship between the individual and the group. I understand you have done some computer modelling of this. Is there something you can share?

And happy birthday!

……………………………………………………………………….
Angus Jenkinson

--
Regards,

Christopher Paul Geddes

Email: <mailto:geddes.chris@gmx.com>geddes.chris@gmail.com
Mobile: +1-876-393-1366

[26.12.17. 15:00 Singapore]

Kent, neither my long delay replying, nor the brevity of this response, reflect the seriousness I have given to your most helpful responses.

I found your remarks on atrocity very profound, especially the suggestion you make: “Is destroying someone’s good name, for instance, an atrocity?�

I agree with your analysis of formal and social games and how these commonly involve cooperation at levels above and below the levels of conflict.

I am intrigued by the how of collective control when each is only controlling their own perception (while anticipating or guessing the other’s). this
is Nobel prize land.

On a separate note, I do have, in a playful way, some reservation with the view that mathematical models are the sine qua non gold standard of analysis,
as in the following…

···

KM: The model goes beyond the verbal description to show the dynamics of the interaction, something the verbal description
can’t really do…

KN: More sophisticated multi-level mathematical models might be able to do a little better job, but it would take an enormously
complex mathematical model to simulate the multi-level complexity of the real-life interaction.

KM: What mathematical models are good for, in my view, is to alert us to the basic patterns of dynamic interaction…

/span>

If a [mathematical] model is required to provide a rigorous analysis and yet the complexity of shared cooperative/conflict reality is too dense to model,
in practice, how is the subject to be understood?

If mathematics is a form of language, as some argue, would it make more sense to say that is hard to achieve any description that does justice to the
complexity of social interaction?

And in what language or thinking form does the “alerting� take place�

………………………………………………€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦……….

Angus Jenkinson

On 10/10/2017, 23:42, “McClelland, Kent” MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU wrote:

From Kent McClelland (2017.10.09.1500 EDT)

Re: Angus Jenkinson (2017-09-26. 23.57), Martin Taylor 2017.09.27.10.40

HI Angus and Martin,

At long last, I’m back in Grinnell and have had some time to think about the questions you asked, Angus. I’ll start with a response to that post and treat Martin’s comments
in another.

AJ: Thank you Kent, I am very grateful to you for sharing this.

I have started studying your paper and I can see the elegance of having a mathematical model for the purposes. But also analytical thinking. I regard it
ass a major contribution. More of which later…

KM: Thanks!

AJ: Although I have worked with conflict, it is not a speciality of mine. So what follows is an enquiry.

KM: My focus on conflict has led me to see it as a ubiquitous feature of social interaction, just as cooperation. collaboration, or coordination of one kind or another occur in almost every interaction. I think of conflict and cooperation,
then, as two varieties of the more general phenomenon of collective control. So, I’d say that anytime you study social interaction you’re implicitly dealing with conflict, and cooperation, too, of course.

KM: Granted that most everyday interactions between people don’t take the form of a conflict escalation spiral, like those described by Collins and analyzed in my paper. The reason that most conflicts don’t get out of hand, as I see it,
is that everyday interactions nearly always require collective control at a variety of perceptual levels simultaneously, and while conflict may be occurring at one perceptual level, cooperation more typically occurs other levels of control. Thus, for us to
do a comprehensive analysis of any given social interaction, we have to think in terms of the participants’ collective control of perceptions on a hierarchy from low-level perceptions of the shared physical environment to the middle-level perceptions involved
in language and communication, to high-level perceptions, such as norms or social identities.

KM: Some examples: Two sports teams are in conflict over which team will win the game, but they cooperate at low levels of perceptual control by playing together on the same field or pitch, using the same ball, etc., and at higher levels
of control by adhering to the perceptual principles defining the rules of the game. In most contests, the conflict between the teams doesn’t ever escalate into unbridled conflict. However, sometimes a member of one team commits what appears to be a deliberate
foul that injures a player on the other team (or just humiliates him). A series of tit-for-tat physical aggressions may then occur as the teams seek to even the moral score, and we see an example of the familiar conflict-escalation pattern.

KM: Two people argue with each other. Cooperative low-level collective control nevertheless occurs as they use a common language and orient their bodies to face each other (although at these low perceptual levels some of their physical
gestures may symbolize threats of violence to the other). While everyday arguments commonly result in patterns of conflict escalation, as the participants trade increasingly cutting insults, for instance, most arguments don’t degenerate into fisticuffs. A
typical reason for this restraint may be that the participants are cooperatively controlling some higher-level perception, perhaps a mutual friendship or relationship that they want to preserve. Another possibility is that both of them recognize and adhere
to higher-level perceptions of norms that prohibit doing violence to another person or prescribe penalties for physical aggression.

KM: In general, the conflict-resolution literature suggests that one good way to resolve a conflict is to focus the combatants’ attention on some goal that they both share, or in other words to get them to cooperatively control some higher-level
perception, rather than conflictively controlling the low-level perceptions that they are fighting about. Interestingly, the MOL method of therapy operates on a similar principle of directing the attention of a client caught in an internal conflict to some
higher-level perception connected to the two lower-level perceptions in conflict, which then enables the client to prioritize which of the incompatible lower-level perceptions to control, or do something else entirely.

KM: Another example: A work team or a committee work together to complete a project or reach some other common goal. The overall tenor of the interaction is cooperative, but along the way lots of little conflicts pop up about the best means
toward reaching their goals. If the group is working together effectively, these conflicts are handled by talking these conflicts out with each other or else deferring to a boss whose word is taken as authoritative and who tells them what to do. The basic
purpose of verbal communication, in my view, is to enable people to align the reference values to be used for their collective control of perceptions. What communication communicates, I would say, are reference values for the perceptions that the speaker wants
the listener to control. People who want to work together cooperatively need well aligned reference values for their common perceptions, and lack of alignment is tantamount to conflict.

  1. AJ: You say: “Of course, every approach to modeling relies on simplifications and abstractions, and degrees of realism exhibited by agent-based models will vary with the level of detail built into the
    modeled agents…â€? You also saay that it does not reproduce the detail of Collins’ explanation. Understood, but one of the cybernetic principles was that the best model was reality itself. Moreover, you comment that Collins does not give precise enough definitions.
    But part of the problem of “definition� is that it tends to reduce the variety of the phenomenon to a boundary defined by the definition. So I am interested in the degree to which you think that real life (sic) can be reproduced in a mathematical model? I
    return to this below…

KM: The highly simplified mathematical models that I used in my paper pertain to only a single level of perception. When multiple levels of perception are in play, as in the examples I’ve suggested above, these simple mathematical models
obviously can’t begin to reproduce the real-life complexity of cooperation on one level of perception and conflict on another. More sophisticated multi-level mathematical models might be able to do a little better job, but it would take an enormously complex
mathematical model to simulate the multi-level complexity of the real-life interaction. What mathematical models are good for, in my view, is to alert us to the basic patterns of dynamic interaction we might expect to see in these real-life instances and also
to provide a theoretically grounded explanation of why such patterns are likely to emerge.

  1. Building on that, as an illustration, you say:
  1. The other core variables in Collins’s model also lack of the kind of conceptual precision needed to support mathematical reformulation. Collins explicitly defines the variable atrocities as a matter
    of the perceptions of people on one side of the conflict interaction: “Atrocities are opponents’ actions that we perceive as especially hurtful and evil . . .� (2012:2). However, he does not offer any operational definition * that
    would enable observers to judge whether a particular action committed by one side is likely to be defined as an atrocity by the other*. My emphasis
  1. One of the conditions of conflict I understand is that the supposedly rational judgement is lost and people tend to find unacceptable, and perhaps extremely so, based on, let’s call it, a biased reaction.
    So what the person deems to be an atrocity might not be seen as such by an observer. That’s easily understood.

KM: Nevertheless, many of the violent atrocities that Collins describes would strike almost any observer as atrocities. When you kill scores of people, for example, that’s an atrocity in anybody’s book. My definition of violence, from a
PCT viewpoint, is the destruction of environmental feedback paths that someone else needs to keep their highly valued perceptions under control. When you kill someone, for instance, you render their physical body nonoperational, and the physical body always
forms two essential links in the circuit of pathways used by individuals to control their perceptions: on the output side, the muscular movements that enable individuals to manipulate their physical environments, and on the input side, the sensory organs that
report the effects of those manipulations. But violence can also consist of destroying physical objects that individuals rely on to control perceptions, like their houses or tools. It’s when you get to higher levels of perception, what Pierre Bourdieu referred
to as symbolic violence, that definitions of what an atrocity might consist of get more subjective. Is destroying someone’s good name, for instance, an atrocity? It’s certainly an essential link in the feedback path that people use to maintain a positive sense
of self.

  1. One of the rediscoveries of neuroscience is the role of emotion in decision making. I am interested in how the strength and roles of those appear in the model?
  1. You refer to reference values as memories (aka mental pictures). So when you say you “will take polarization to refer to the extent of differences in reference values�, I take that to mean that the
    (actively filtered and probably distorted) perception is compared by each party to something conjured from their own memories (forgive the unmathematical description).
  1. You also say, “PCT models show the output of the two interacting systems diverging, with one system, in effect, pulling in one direction to bring the environmental variable into line with its preferences,
    while the other system pulls in the other (see McClelland 2004, 2006). Hence, the variable used in my simulations is the intensity of the conflict, measured by the degree of divergence in system outputs.�Outputs are to the control variable of the other as
    I understand it.

KM: As Martin notes in his reply, one’s outputs affect the shared environmental reality, and the opponent controls a separate perception based on that environmental reality, but that perception isn’t necessarily based on the opponent’s
perception of the individual’s output. What happens in conflict is that a positive feedback loop emerges that links the two opponents’ outputs, the portions of the environment on which both outputs are acting, and their respective perceptual control loops.

  1. So how do you process emotion in the model?

KM: Martin’s reply, directing you to Ch 17 in the second edition of Behavior: The Control of Perception, is a good start for answering the question about the place of emotion in this model. What I’d add is that Powers argues that the experience
of prolonged inability to bring a valued perception under control inevitably produces negative emotions like frustration and anger. In conflict situations, the actions of the opponent by definition prevent one from bringing the contested perceptual variable
into control at one’s own reference level, so the PCT definition of conflict implies that these situations always produce negative emotions. Emotion is just an expected part of conflict. The other thing I would say is that because people are operating on several
different levels of perception at the same time, emotional reactions do not preclude rational behavior. The two processes can go on simultaneously.

  1. How do you see the model contributing to practitioner work? And what other variables would need to be included? Is it more than offering a conceptual framework to think differently? Meaning, how would
    a version be used in Trump v Kim JU?

KM: The mud-slinging match between Trump and Kim sure looks like a classic conflict-escalation cycle to me. What my model does not do is predict the outcome of this escalating conflict, because it doesn’t take into account the other presumably
higher-level perceptions that these two actors may be controlling. Let’s hope that there are some perceptions that restrain them before they start pushing nuclear buttons!

  1. It seems that being able to prove similar results to positive feedbacks through negative feedback is very useful in rethinking what happens.
  1. It also seems to me it replicates what practitioners in conflict resolution do. They ask each party to see how the other party sees things. Negotiators teach the same thing.

KM: Good point.

  1. Might however the +ve and –ve be mirrors of each other accordiing to how one analysis the situation? One is dealing with a series of escalating outcomes that propel each other and the other a series
    of negative (internal) cancellations that also effectively propel each other? Your strength is to show the interaction, maybe?

KM: I’m not quite sure what you mean by the ±ve in the model. The value of the environmental variable [v] times what [e]?

  1. The paper also introduces some clarity in thinking about issues. This is a typical case in all model building and as long as one does not allow a spurious impression of detail to get in the way, can
    help. I like for example the parsimonious description: “From the PCT perspective, conflict occurs whenever the two sides use different reference standards in their attempts to control an environmental variable, and victory or defeat in the contest is represented
    by the extent to which side or the other succeeds in bringing the variable into line with its own preferred reference conditions�.
  1. I wonder whether it does not verbally – as a result of your thinking tthrough the issues with the help of modelling – give the practitioner as good an insight as a mathematical model?

KM: The model goes beyond the verbal description to show the dynamics of the interaction, something the verbal description can’t really do.

BTW: You mention: ““reflex arc� as conventionally understood�; which version of this do you mean? Presumably not the one that Dewey critiqued?

KM: No, I am talking about Dewey’s reflex arc, as well as the set of internal neural connections that provides the focus of most current neurological analyses.

KM: Thanks again for your good questions and your interest in my work!

Kent

A

On 24/09/2017, 20:20, “McClelland, Kent” MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU wrote:

[from Kent McClelland 2017.09.24.1735]

Hi Angus (and Christopher),

I’ve done a lot of thinking and some PCT-based modeling that relates to questions of collective action, organizations, and, more broadly, how society is possible.

The modeling I’ve done may strike you as pretty simplistic for your purposes, but the best summary of my work along that line can be found in a paper published in 2014 in the journal Sociological Theory called “Cycles
of Conflict: A Computational Alternative to Collins’s Theory of Conflict Escalation.” You can download a copy of this paper, as well as my other published papers on PCT, from my page in ResearchGate.

A much longer and more detailed, but strictly theoretical, exposition of my ideas about “collective control”, as I’ve called it, is contained in my chapter for the Living Control Systems IV volume being published
by Bill Powers’s sister, Alice. The chapter is called “Social Structure and Control: Perceptual Control Theory and the Science of Sociology.”

Unfortunately, the LCS IV volume has been slow to appear and still remains unpublished. I don’t know when it will be out, but the publisher has requested that authors not distribute any draft copies of the chapters
prior to publication, so I can’t share my chapter with you at this point. I’ve posted a summary of the chapter on ResearchGate, but it’s probably too condensed to provide any very good sense of my argument.

Martin Taylor’s contribution to the LCS IV volume may also be of interest to you when that book appears. I’ve seen some early drafts of his chapters, and many of his ideas were immediately influential to me as I put my own chapter together. He comes at the
question of how to apply PCT to social interaction from a somewhat different angle than I do, but his work is unlike anything else I’ve seen, and I think it will prove foundational for lots of future applications of PCT in this area.

Please send me some examples of your work if there are things you’d like to share. I’m definitely interested.

And thanks to all for the birthday greetings! I’ve had a fun birthday going with two of my young grandsons to the chocolate museum in Hamburg, Germany.

Kent

On Sep 24, 2017, at 3:08 PM, Christopher Paul Geddes geddes.chris@gmail.com wrote:

Angus,

Thanks for writing. I hope all is well. I have done some modeling in the past however I was kind of limited and now I am more focused on Systems Dynamics which is making me take more of an interest in modeling now
as well.

I would like to know more about what you are looking for if I can help. I like to think of organizations as entities that I can influence with technology so that things so the way the team wants them to go.

Christopher

On Sun, Sep 24, 2017 at 5:48 AM, Angus Jenkinson angus@angusjenkinson.com wrote:

[Angus Jenkinson, 2017-09-23.11.46]

Kent, Bruce Nevin has suggested that I should reach out to you in relationship to some work on modelling that you have been doing on collective behaviour.
I am interested, and working with, the behaviour of organisations and social groups and the relationship between the individual and the group. I understand you have done some computer modelling of this. Is there something you can share?

And happy birthday!

……………………………………………………………………………….

iv>
Angus Jenkinson

Regards,

Christopher Paul Geddes

Email: geddes.chris@gmail.com

Mobile: +1-876-393-1366

[From Bruce Nevin (2017.12.26.11:13 ET)]

Angus Jenkinson (26.12.17. 15:00 Singapore) –

There’s some equivocation lurking here.

AJ: If mathematics is a form of language, as some argue, would it make more sense to say that [it] is hard to achieve any description that does justice to the complexity of social interaction?

AJ: And in what language or thinking form does the “alertingâ€? take placeâ€?

Mathematics: the notational conventions, rules of manipulation, and so on are derived from language, just as any formal mathematical expression can be ‘read out’ in ordinary language, and routinely is. (I can say more about this if necessary.) Mathematics: the patterns, relationships, etc. so notationally represented are perceptions independent of language. An experienced mathematician knows the challenge of putting a mathematical ‘intuition’ into demonstrable form. You may recall how C.S. Peirce inveighed against the tidy, pyramidal proof, preferring an account of how the mathematician actually arrived at his or her conclusion.

KM: What mathematical models are good for, in my view, is to alert us to the basic patterns of dynamic interaction

<

The ‘alerting’ is in the form of mathematical perceptions of “the basic patterns of dynamic interaction”, facilitated or sometimes merely hinted by the language-like notational expressions with which a mathematical model is presented.

···

On Tue, Dec 26, 2017 at 3:31 AM, Angus Jenkinson angus@angusjenkinson.com wrote:

[26.12.17. 15:00 Singapore]

Â

Kent, neither my long delay replying, nor the brevity of this response, reflect the seriousness I have given to your most helpful responses.

Â

I found your remarks on atrocity very profound, especially the suggestion you make: “Is destroying someone’s good name, for instance, an atrocity?â€?

Â

I agree with your analysis of formal and social games and how these commonly involve cooperation at levels above and below the levels of conflict.

Â

I am intrigued by the how of collective control when each is only controlling their own perception (while anticipating or guessing the other’s). this
is Nobel prize land.

Â

On a separate note, I do have, in a playful way, some reservation with the view that mathematical models are the sine qua non gold standard of analysis,
as in the following…

Â

KM: The model goes beyond the verbal description to show the dynamics of the interaction, something the verbal description
can’t really do…

KN: More sophisticated multi-level mathematical models might be able to do a little better job, but it would take an enormously
complex mathematical model to simulate the multi-level complexity of the real-life interaction.

KM: What mathematical models are good for, in my view, is to alert us to the basic patterns of dynamic interaction…

Â

If a [mathematical] model is required to provide a rigorous analysis and yet the complexity of shared cooperative/conflict reality is too dense to model,
in practice, how is the subject to be understood?

If mathematics is a form of language, as some argue, would it make more sense to say that is hard to achieve any description that does justice to the
complexity of social interaction?

And in what language or thinking form does the “alertingâ€? take placeâ€?

Â

………€¦â€¦……………………………………………………… ¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦.

Angus Jenkinson

Â

Â

Â

On 10/10/2017, 23:42, “McClelland, Kent” MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU wrote:

Â

From Kent McClelland (2017.10.09.1500 EDT)

Â

Re: Angus Jenkinson (2017-09-26. 23.57), Martin Taylor 2017.09.27.10.40

Â

HI Angus and Martin,

Â

At long last, I’m back in Grinnell and have had some time to think about the questions you asked, Angus. I’ll start with a response to that post and treat Martin’s comments
in another.

Â

AJ: Thank you Kent, I am very grateful to you for sharing this.

Â

I have started studying your paper and I can see the elegance of having a mathematical model for the purposes. But also analytical thinking. I regard it
ass a major contribution. More of which later…>

Â

KM: Thanks!

Â

AJ: Although I have worked with conflict, it is not a speciality of mine. So what follows is an enquiry.

Â

KM: My focus on conflict has led me to see it as a ubiquitous feature of social interaction, just as cooperation. collaboration, or coordination of one kind or another occur in almost every interaction. I think of conflict and cooperation,
then, as two varieties of the more general phenomenon of collective control. So, I’d say that anytime you study social interaction you’re implicitly dealing with conflict, and cooperation, too, of course.Â

Â

KM: Granted that most everyday interactions between people don’t take the form of a conflict escalation spiral, like those described by Collins and analyzed in my paper. The reason that most conflicts don’t get out of hand, as I see it,
is that everyday interactions nearly always require collective control at a variety of perceptual levels simultaneously, and while conflict may be occurring at one perceptual level, cooperation more typically occurs other levels of control. Thus, for us to
do a comprehensive analysis of any given social interaction, we have to think in terms of the participants’ collective control of perceptions on a hierarchy from low-level perceptions of the shared physical environment to the middle-level perceptions involved
in language and communication, to high-level perceptions, such as norms or social identities.

Â

KM: Some examples: Two sports teams are in conflict over which team will win the game, but they cooperate at low levels of perceptual control by playing together on the same field or pitch, using the same ball, etc., and at higher levels
of control by adhering to the perceptual principles defining the rules of the game. In most contests, the conflict between the teams doesn’t ever escalate into unbridled conflict. However, sometimes a member of one team commits what appears to be a deliberate
foul that injures a player on the other team (or just humiliates him). A series of tit-for-tat physical aggressions may then occur as the teams seek to even the moral score, and we see an example of the familiar conflict-escalation pattern.Â

Â

KM: Two people argue with each other. Cooperative low-level collective control nevertheless occurs as they use a common language and orient their bodies to face each other (although at these low perceptual levels some of their physical
gestures may symbolize threats of violence to the other). While everyday arguments commonly result in patterns of conflict escalation, as the participants trade increasingly cutting insults, for instance, most arguments don’t degenerate into fisticuffs. A
typical reason for this restraint may be that the participants are cooperatively controlling some higher-level perception, perhaps a mutual friendship or relationship that they want to preserve. Another possibility is that both of them recognize and adhere
to higher-level perceptions of norms that prohibit doing violence to another person or prescribe penalties for physical aggression.Â

Â

KM: In general, the conflict-resolution literature suggests that one good way to resolve a conflict is to focus the combatants’ attention on some goal that they both share, or in other words to get them to cooperatively control some higher-level
perception, rather than conflictively controlling the low-level perceptions that they are fighting about. Interestingly, the MOL method of therapy operates on a similar principle of directing the attention of a client caught in an internal conflict to some
higher-level perception connected to the two lower-level perceptions in conflict, which then enables the client to prioritize which of the incompatible lower-level perceptions to control, or do something else entirely.Â

Â

KM: Another example: A work team or a committee work together to complete a project or reach some other common goal. The overall tenor of the interaction is cooperative, but along the way lots of little conflicts pop up about the best means
toward reaching their goals. If the group is working together effectively, these conflicts are handled by talking these conflicts out with each other or else deferring to a boss whose word is taken as authoritative and who tells them what to do. The basic
purpose of verbal communication, in my view, is to enable people to align the reference values to be used for their collective control of perceptions. What communication communicates, I would say, are reference values for the perceptions that the speaker wants
the listener to control. People who want to work together cooperatively need well aligned reference values for their common perceptions, and lack of alignment is tantamount to conflict.Â

Â

  1. AJ: You say: “Of course, every approach to modeling relies on simplifications and abstractions, and degrees of realism exhibited by agent-based models will vary with the level of detail built into the
    modeled agents…â€? You also say that it does not reproduce tthe detail of Collins’ explanation. Understood, but one of the cybernetic principles was that the best model was reality itself. Moreover, you comment that Collins does not give precise enough definitions.
    But part of the problem of “definitionâ€? is that it tends to reduce the variety of the phenomenon to a boundary defined by the definition. So I am interested in the degree to which you think that real life (sic) can be reproduced in a mathematical model? I
    return to this below…

Â

KM: The highly simplified mathematical models that I used in my paper pertain to only a single level of perception. When multiple levels of perception are in play, as in the examples I’ve suggested above, these simple mathematical models
obviously can’t begin to reproduce the real-life complexity of cooperation on one level of perception and conflict on another. More sophisticated multi-level mathematical models might be able to do a little better job, but it would take an enormously complex
mathematical model to simulate the multi-level complexity of the real-life interaction. What mathematical models are good for, in my view, is to alert us to the basic patterns of dynamic interaction we might expect to see in these real-life instances and also
to provide a theoretically grounded explanation of why such patterns are likely to emerge.

  1. Â
  2. Building on that, as an illustration, you say:
  3. The other core variables in Collins’s model also lack of the kind of conceptual precision needed to support mathematical reformulation. Collins explicitly defines the variable atrocities as a matter
    of the perceptions of people on one side of the conflict interaction: “Atrocities are opponents’ actions that we perceive as especially hurtful and evil . . .â€? (2012:2). However, he does not offer any operational definition * that
    would enable observers to judge whether a particular action committed by one side is likely to be defined as an atrocity by the other*. My emphasis
  4. One of the conditions of conflict I understand is that the supposedly rational judgement is lost and people tend to find unacceptable, and perhaps extremely so, based on, let’s call it, a biased reaction.
    So what the person deems to be an atrocity might not be seen as such by an observer. That’s easily understood.Â

Â

KM: Nevertheless, many of the violent atrocities that Collins describes would strike almost any observer as atrocities. When you kill scores of people, for example, that’s an atrocity in anybody’s book. My definition of violence, from a
PCT viewpoint, is the destruction of environmental feedback paths that someone else needs to keep their highly valued perceptions under control. When you kill someone, for instance, you render their physical body nonoperational, and the physical body always
forms two essential links in the circuit of pathways used by individuals to control their perceptions: on the output side, the muscular movements that enable individuals to manipulate their physical environments, and on the input side, the sensory organs that
report the effects of those manipulations. But violence can also consist of destroying physical objects that individuals rely on to control perceptions, like their houses or tools. It’s when you get to higher levels of perception, what Pierre Bourdieu referred
to as symbolic violence, that definitions of what an atrocity might consist of get more subjective. Is destroying someone’s good name, for instance, an atrocity? It’s certainly an essential link in the feedback path that people use to maintain a positive sense
of self.Â

  1. Â
  2. One of the rediscoveries of neuroscience is the role of emotion in decision making. I am interested in how the strength and roles of those appear in the model?
  3. You refer to reference values as memories (aka mental pictures). So when you say you “will take polarization to refer to the extent of differences in reference valuesâ€?, I take that to mean that the
    (actively filtered and probably distorted) perception is compared by each party to something conjured from their own memories (forgive the unmathematical description).
  4. You also say, “PCT models show the output of the two interacting systems diverging, with one system, in effect, pulling in one direction to bring the environmental variable into line with its preferences,
    while the other system pulls in the other (see McClelland 2004, 2006). Hence, the variable used in my simulations is the intensity of the conflict, measured by the degree of divergence in system outputs.�Outputs are to the control variable of the other as
    I understand it.Â

Â

KM: As Martin notes in his reply, one’s outputs affect the shared environmental reality, and the opponent controls a separate perception based on that environmental reality, but that perception isn’t necessarily based on the opponent’s
perception of the individual’s output. What happens in conflict is that a positive feedback loop emerges that links the two opponents’ outputs, the portions of the environment on which both outputs are acting, and their respective perceptual control loops.

  1. Â
  2. So how do you process emotion in the model? Â

Â

KM: Martin’s reply, directing you to Ch 17 in the second edition of Behavior: The Control of Perception, is a good start for answering the question about the place of emotion in this model. What I’d add is that Powers argues that the experience
of prolonged inability to bring a valued perception under control inevitably produces negative emotions like frustration and anger. In conflict situations, the actions of the opponent by definition prevent one from bringing the contested perceptual variable
into control at one’s own reference level, so the PCT definition of conflict implies that these situations always produce negative emotions. Emotion is just an expected part of conflict. The other thing I would say is that because people are operating on several
different levels of perception at the same time, emotional reactions do not preclude rational behavior. The two processes can go on simultaneously.Â

Â

  1. Â
  2. How do you see the model contributing to practitioner work? And what other variables would need to be included? Is it more than offering a conceptual framework to think differently? Meaning, how would
    a version be used in Trump v Kim JU?

Â

KM: The mud-slinging match between Trump and Kim sure looks like a classic conflict-escalation cycle to me. What my model does not do is predict the outcome of this escalating conflict, because it doesn’t take into account the other presumably
higher-level perceptions that these two actors may be controlling. Let’s hope that there are some perceptions that restrain them before they start pushing nuclear buttons!

  1. Â
  2. It seems that being able to prove similar results to positive feedbacks through negative feedback is very useful in rethinking what happens.Â
  3. It also seems to me it replicates what practitioners in conflict resolution do. They ask each party to see how the other party sees things. Negotiators teach the same thing.

Â

KM: Good point.

  1. Â
  2. Might however the +ve and –ve be mirrors of each other according to howw one analysis the situation? One is dealing with a series of escalating outcomes that propel each other and the other a series
    of negative (internal) cancellations that also effectively propel each other? Your strength is to show the interaction, maybe?

Â

KM: I’m not quite sure what you mean by the ±ve in the model. The value of the environmental variable [v] times what [e]?

  1. Â
  2. The paper also introduces some clarity in thinking about issues. This is a typical case in all model building and as long as one does not allow a spurious impression of detail to get in the way, can
    help. I like for example the parsimonious description: “From the PCT perspective, conflict occurs whenever the two sides use different reference standards in their attempts to control an environmental variable, and victory or defeat in the contest is represented
    by the extent to which side or the other succeeds in bringing the variable into line with its own preferred reference conditionsâ€?.Â
  3. I wonder whether it does not verbally – as a result of your thinking thhrough the issues with the help of modelling – give the practitionerr as good an insight as a mathematical model?

Â

KM: The model goes beyond the verbal description to show the dynamics of the interaction, something the verbal description can’t really do.Â

  1. Â
    Â

BTW: You mention: ““reflex arcâ€? as conventionally understoodâ€?; which version of this do you mean? Presumably not the one that Dewey critiqued?Â

Â

KM: No, I am talking about Dewey’s reflex arc, as well as the set of internal neural connections that provides the focus of most current neurological analyses.Â

Â

KM: Thanks again for your good questions and your interest in my work!

Â

Kent

Â

—Â

A

Â

Â

On 24/09/2017, 20:20, “McClelland, Kent” MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU wrote:

Â

[from Kent McClelland 2017.09.24.1735]

Â

Hi Angus (and Christopher),

Â

I’ve done a lot of thinking and some PCT-based modeling that relates to questions of collective action, organizations, and, more broadly, how society is possible.Â

Â

The modeling I’ve done may strike you as pretty simplistic for your purposes, but the best summary of my work along that line can be found in a paper published in 2014 in the journal Sociological Theory called “Cycles
of Conflict: A Computational Alternative to Collins’s Theory of Conflict Escalation.” You can download a copy of this paper, as well as my other published papers on PCT, from my page in ResearchGate.

Â

A much longer and more detailed, but strictly theoretical, exposition of my ideas about “collective control”, as I’ve called it, is contained in my chapter for the Living Control Systems IV volume being published
by Bill Powers’s sister, Alice. The chapter is called "Social Structure and Control: Perceptual Control Theory and the Science of Sociology."Â

Â

Unfortunately, the LCS IV volume has been slow to appear and still remains unpublished. I don’t know when it will be out, but the publisher has requested that authors not distribute any draft copies of the chapters
prior to publication, so I can’t share my chapter with you at this point. I’ve posted a summary of the chapter on ResearchGate, but it’s probably too condensed to provide any very good sense of my argument.

Martin Taylor’s contribution to the LCS IV volume may also be of interest to you when that book appears. I’ve seen some early drafts of his chapters, and many of his ideas were immediately influential to me as I put my own chapter together. He comes at the
question of how to apply PCT to social interaction from a somewhat different angle than I do, but his work is unlike anything else I’ve seen, and I think it will prove foundational for lots of future applications of PCT in this area.Â

Â

Please send me some examples of your work if there are things you’d like to share. I’m definitely interested.Â

And thanks to all for the birthday greetings! I’ve had a fun birthday going with two of my young grandsons to the chocolate museum in Hamburg, Germany.

Â

KentÂ

On Sep 24, 2017, at 3:08 PM, Christopher Paul Geddes geddes.chris@gmail.com wrote:

Angus,Â

Â

Thanks for writing. I hope all is well. I have done some modeling in the past however I was kind of limited and now I am more focused on Systems Dynamics which is making me take more of an interest in modeling now
as well.Â

Â

I would like to know more about what you are looking for if I can help. I like to think of organizations as entities that I can influence with technology so that things so the way the team wants them to go.

Â

Christopher

Â

On Sun, Sep 24, 2017 at 5:48 AM, Angus Jenkinson angus@angusjenkinson.com wrote:

[Angus Jenkinson, 2017-09-23.11.46]

Â

Kent, Bruce Nevin has suggested that I should reach out to you in relationship to some work on modelling that you have been doing on collective behaviour.Â
I am interested, and working with, the behaviour of organisations and social groups and the relationship between the individual and the group. I understand you have done some computer modelling of this. Is there something you can share?

Â

And happy birthday!

Â

……………………………………………………… ¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦……….

/p>

Angus Jenkinson

Â

–Â

Regards,

Â

Christopher Paul Geddes

Â

Email:Â geddes.chris@gmail.com

Mobile: +1-876-393-1366

Â

Â

Â

from Kent McClelland (2017.12.26 16:30)
Angus Jenkinson [26.12.17. 15:00 Singapore]
KM: Thank you, Angus, for your kind words about my earlier post. A couple of comments below.

AJ: Kent, neither my long delay replying, nor the brevity of this response, reflect the seriousness I have given to your most helpful responses.

I found your remarks on atrocity very profound, especially the suggestion you make: “Is destroying someone’s good name, for instance, an atrocity?�

I agree with your analysis of formal and social games and how these commonly involve cooperation at levels above and below the levels of conflict.

I am intrigued by the how of collective control when each is only controlling their own perception (while anticipating or guessing the other’s). this is Nobel prize land.

KM: High praise, indeed, but I’m not holding my breath for a call from the Nobel committee!

AJ: On a separate note, I do have, in a playful way, some reservation with the view that mathematical models are the sine qua non gold standard of analysis, as in the following…&nnbsp;

KM: The model goes beyond the verbal description to show the dynamics of the interaction, something the verbal description can’t really do…
KN: More sophisticated multi-level mathematical models might be able to do a little better job, but it would take an enormously complex mathematical model to simulate the multi-level complexity of the real-life interaction.
KM: What mathematical models are good for, in my view, is to alert us to the basic patterns of dynamic interaction…

If a [mathematical] model is required to provide a rigorous analysis and yet the complexity of shared cooperative/conflict reality is too dense to model, in practice, how is the subject to be understood?
If mathematics is a form of language, as some argue, would it make more sense to say that is hard to achieve any description that does justice to the complexity of social interaction?
And in what language or thinking form does the “alerting� take place�

KM: My reason for getting involved in mathematical modeling was that the dynamics of control systems in action and interaction weren’t obvious to me when I first encountered PCT, and so it took a lot of playing around with models for me to get a clear sense of how control systems work. Maybe a person can gain a deep understanding of PCT without trying out any mathematical models, but some hands-on tinkering with models really helps, in my experience.
If you’ve made an intellectual commitment, as I have, to the proposition that the dynamics of control systems are our best conceptual model for understanding the dynamics of human behavior, then you need to have a thorough knowledge of how control systems actually work in order to know what to look for in examining empirical instances of human behavior. I don’t pretend that I’ll ever be able to model a strip of human interaction in all its real-time complexity, but some basic modeling seems really helpful for cutting through some of that complexity.
I would say that the reason for using a model of any kind—mathematical or conceptual—is to hi highlight significant patterns that might be possible to pull out of the messy complexity of an empirical phenomenon. Of course, models can never be more than abstractions and simplifications of the reality to which they are applied, but for scientists (and folks like me who try to think scientifically) mathematical models are particularly useful because they provide such a compact and rigorous way of communicating scientific descriptions of empirical phenomena. In comparison, ordinary language tends to be fuzzy and imprecise.
Does this speak to your reservations about modeling, Angus?
Kent

………………â¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦………………………………….
Angus Jenkinson

From Kent McClelland (2017.10.09.1500 EDT)

Re: Angus Jenkinson (2017-09-26. 23.57), Martin Taylor 2017.09.27.10.40

HI Angus and Martin,

At long last, I’m back in Grinnell and have had some time to think about the questions you asked, Angus. I’ll start with a response to that post and treat Martin’s comments in another.

AJ: Thank you Kent, I am very grateful to you for sharing this.

I have started studying your paper and I can see the elegance of having a mathematical model for the purposes. But also analytical thinking. I regard it ass a major contribution. More of which later…

KM: Thanks!

AJ: Although I have worked with conflict, it is not a speciality of mine. So what follows is an enquiry.

KM: My focus on conflict has led me to see it as a ubiquitous feature of social interaction, just as cooperation. collaboration, or coordination of one kind or another occur in almost every interaction. I think of conflict and cooperation, then, as two varieties of the more general phenomenon of collective control. So, I’d say that anytime you study social interaction you’re implicitly dealing with conflict, and cooperation, too, of course.

KM: Granted that most everyday interactions between people don’t take the form of a conflict escalation spiral, like those described by Collins and analyzed in my paper. The reason that most conflicts don’t get out of hand, as I see it, is that everyday interactions nearly always require collective control at a variety of perceptual levels simultaneously, and while conflict may be occurring at one perceptual level, cooperation more typically occurs other levels of control. Thus, for us to do a comprehensive analysis of any given social interaction, we have to think in terms of the participants' collective control of perceptions on a hierarchy from low-level perceptions of the shared physical environment to the middle-level perceptions involved in language and communication, to high-level perceptions, such as norms or social identities.

KM: Some examples: Two sports teams are in conflict over which team will win the game, but they cooperate at low levels of perceptual control by playing together on the same field or pitch, using the same ball, etc., and at higher levels of control by adhering to the perceptual principles defining the rules of the game. In most contests, the conflict between the teams doesn’t ever escalate into unbridled conflict. However, sometimes a member of one team commits what appears to be a deliberate foul that injures a player on the other team (or just humiliates him). A series of tit-for-tat physical aggressions may then occur as the teams seek to even the moral score, and we see an example of the familiar conflict-escalation pattern.

KM: Two people argue with each other. Cooperative low-level collective control nevertheless occurs as they use a common language and orient their bodies to face each other (although at these low perceptual levels some of their physical gestures may symbolize threats of violence to the other). While everyday arguments commonly result in patterns of conflict escalation, as the participants trade increasingly cutting insults, for instance, most arguments don’t degenerate into fisticuffs. A typical reason for this restraint may be that the participants are cooperatively controlling some higher-level perception, perhaps a mutual friendship or relationship that they want to preserve. Another possibility is that both of them recognize and adhere to higher-level perceptions of norms that prohibit doing violence to another person or prescribe penalties for physical aggression.

KM: In general, the conflict-resolution literature suggests that one good way to resolve a conflict is to focus the combatants' attention on some goal that they both share, or in other words to get them to cooperatively control some higher-level perception, rather than conflictively controlling the low-level perceptions that they are fighting about. Interestingly, the MOL method of therapy operates on a similar principle of directing the attention of a client caught in an internal conflict to some higher-level perception connected to the two lower-level perceptions in conflict, which then enables the client to prioritize which of the incompatible lower-level perceptions to control, or do something else entirely.

KM: Another example: A work team or a committee work together to complete a project or reach some other common goal. The overall tenor of the interaction is cooperative, but along the way lots of little conflicts pop up about the best means toward reaching their goals. If the group is working together effectively, these conflicts are handled by talking these conflicts out with each other or else deferring to a boss whose word is taken as authoritative and who tells them what to do. The basic purpose of verbal communication, in my view, is to enable people to align the reference values to be used for their collective control of perceptions. What communication communicates, I would say, are reference values for the perceptions that the speaker wants the listener to control. People who want to work together cooperatively need well aligned reference values for their common perceptions, and lack of alignment is tantamount to conflict.

1. AJ: You say: “Of course, every approach to modeling relies on simplifications and abstractions, and degrees of realism exhibited by agent-based models will vary with the level of detail built into the modeled agents…â€? You also say that it doees not reproduce the detail of Collins’ explanation. Understood, but one of the cybernetic principles was that the best model was reality itself. Moreover, you comment that Collins does not give precise enough definitions. But part of the problem of “definitionâ€? is that it tends to reduce the variety of the phenomenon to a boundary defined by the definition. So I am interested in the degree to which you think that real life (sic) can be reproduced in a mathematical model? I return to this below…

KM: The highly simplified mathematical models that I used in my paper pertain to only a single level of perception. When multiple levels of perception are in play, as in the examples I’ve suggested above, these simple mathematical models obviously can’t begin to reproduce the real-life complexity of cooperation on one level of perception and conflict on another. More sophisticated multi-level mathematical models might be able to do a little better job, but it would take an enormously complex mathematical model to simulate the multi-level complexity of the real-life interaction. What mathematical models are good for, in my view, is to alert us to the basic patterns of dynamic interaction we might expect to see in these real-life instances and also to provide a theoretically grounded explanation of why such patterns are likely to emerge.

1.

2. Building on that, as an illustration, you say:
  1. The other core variables in Collins’s model also lack of the kind of conceptual precision needed to support mathematical reformulation. Collins explicitly defines the variable atrocities as a matter of the perceptions of people on one side of the conflict interaction: “Atrocities are opponents’ actions that we perceive as especially hurtful and evil . . .â€? (2012:2). However, he does not offer any operational definition that would enable observers to judge whether a particular action committed by one side is likely to be defined as an atrocity by the other. My emphasis
  2. One of the conditions of conflict I understand is that the supposedly rational judgement is lost and people tend to find unacceptable, and perhaps extremely so, based on, let’s call it, a biased reaction. So what the person deems to be an atrocity might not be seen as such by an observer. That’s easily understood.

KM: Nevertheless, many of the violent atrocities that Collins describes would strike almost any observer as atrocities. When you kill scores of people, for example, that’s an atrocity in anybody’s book. My definition of violence, from a PCT viewpoint, is the destruction of environmental feedback paths that someone else needs to keep their highly valued perceptions under control. When you kill someone, for instance, you render their physical body nonoperational, and the physical body always forms two essential links in the circuit of pathways used by individuals to control their perceptions: on the output side, the muscular movements that enable individuals to manipulate their physical environments, and on the input side, the sensory organs that report the effects of those manipulations. But violence can also consist of destroying physical objects that individuals rely on to control perceptions, like their houses or tools. It’s when you get to higher levels of perception, what Pierre Bourdieu referred to as symbolic violence, that definitions of what an atrocity might consist of get more subjective. Is destroying someone’s good name, for instance, an atrocity? It’s certainly an essential link in the feedback path that people use to maintain a positive sense of self.

  1.

1. One of the rediscoveries of neuroscience is the role of emotion in decision making. I am interested in how the strength and roles of those appear in the model?
  1. You refer to reference values as memories (aka mental pictures). So when you say you “will take polarization to refer to the extent of differences in reference valuesâ€?, I take that to mean that the (actively filtered and probably distorted) perception is compared by each party to something conjured from their own memories (forgive the unmathematical description).
  2. You also say, “PCT models show the output of the two interacting systems diverging, with one system, in effect, pulling in one direction to bring the environmental variable into line with its preferences, while the other system pulls in the other (see McClelland 2004, 2006). Hence, the variable used in my simulations is the intensity of the conflict, measured by the degree of divergence in system outputs.â€?Outputs are to the control variable of the other as I understand it.

KM: As Martin notes in his reply, one’s outputs affect the shared environmental reality, and the opponent controls a separate perception based on that environmental reality, but that perception isn’t necessarily based on the opponent’s perception of the individual’s output. What happens in conflict is that a positive feedback loop emerges that links the two opponents’ outputs, the portions of the environment on which both outputs are acting, and their respective perceptual control loops.

  1.

  2. So how do you process emotion in the model?

KM: Martin’s reply, directing you to Ch 17 in the second edition of Behavior: The Control of Perception, is a good start for answering the question about the place of emotion in this model. What I’d add is that Powers argues that the experience of prolonged inability to bring a valued perception under control inevitably produces negative emotions like frustration and anger. In conflict situations, the actions of the opponent by definition prevent one from bringing the contested perceptual variable into control at one’s own reference level, so the PCT definition of conflict implies that these situations always produce negative emotions. Emotion is just an expected part of conflict. The other thing I would say is that because people are operating on several different levels of perception at the same time, emotional reactions do not preclude rational behavior. The two processes can go on simultaneously.

  1.

1. How do you see the model contributing to practitioner work? And what other variables would need to be included? Is it more than offering a conceptual framework to think differently? Meaning, how would a version be used in Trump v Kim JU?

KM: The mud-slinging match between Trump and Kim sure looks like a classic conflict-escalation cycle to me. What my model does not do is predict the outcome of this escalating conflict, because it doesn’t take into account the other presumably higher-level perceptions that these two actors may be controlling. Let’s hope that there are some perceptions that restrain them before they start pushing nuclear buttons!

1.

  1. It seems that being able to prove similar results to positive feedbacks through negative feedback is very useful in rethinking what happens.
  2. It also seems to me it replicates what practitioners in conflict resolution do. They ask each party to see how the other party sees things. Negotiators teach the same thing.

KM: Good point.

  1.

  2. Might however the +ve and –ve be mirrors of each other according too how one analysis the situation? One is dealing with a series of escalating outcomes that propel each other and the other a series of negative (internal) cancellations that also effectively propel each other? Your strength is to show the interaction, maybe?

KM: I’m not quite sure what you mean by the ±ve in the model. The value of the environmental variable [v] times what [e]?

  1.

  2. The paper also introduces some clarity in thinking about issues. This is a typical case in all model building and as long as one does not allow a spurious impression of detail to get in the way, can help. I like for example the parsimonious description: “From the PCT perspective, conflict occurs whenever the two sides use different reference standards in their attempts to control an environmental variable, and victory or defeat in the contest is represented by the extent to which side or the other succeeds in bringing the variable into line with its own preferred reference conditionsâ€?.
  3. I wonder whether it does not verbally – as a result of your thinkking through the issues with the help of modelling – give the practitioner as good an inssight as a mathematical model?

KM: The model goes beyond the verbal description to show the dynamics of the interaction, something the verbal description can’t really do.

  1.

BTW: You mention: ““reflex arc� as conventionally understood�; which version of this do you mean? Presumably not the one that Dewey critiqued?

KM: No, I am talking about Dewey’s reflex arc, as well as the set of internal neural connections that provides the focus of most current neurological analyses.

KM: Thanks again for your good questions and your interest in my work!

Kent

···

On 10/10/2017, 23:42, "McClelland, Kent" <<mailto:MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU>MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU> wrote:


A

On 24/09/2017, 20:20, "McClelland, Kent" <<mailto:MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU>MCCLEL@Grinnell.EDU> wrote:

[from Kent McClelland 2017.09.24.1735]

Hi Angus (and Christopher),

I've done a lot of thinking and some PCT-based modeling that relates to questions of collective action, organizations, and, more broadly, how society is possible.

The modeling I've done may strike you as pretty simplistic for your purposes, but the best summary of my work along that line can be found in a paper published in 2014 in the journal Sociological Theory called "Cycles of Conflict: A Computational Alternative to Collins's Theory of Conflict Escalation." You can download a copy of this paper, as well as my other published papers on PCT, from my page in ResearchGate.

A much longer and more detailed, but strictly theoretical, exposition of my ideas about "collective control", as I've called it, is contained in my chapter for the Living Control Systems IV volume being published by Bill Powers's sister, Alice. The chapter is called "Social Structure and Control: Perceptual Control Theory and the Science of Sociology."

Unfortunately, the LCS IV volume has been slow to appear and still remains unpublished. I don't know when it will be out, but the publisher has requested that authors not distribute any draft copies of the chapters prior to publication, so I can't share my chapter with you at this point. I've posted a summary of the chapter on ResearchGate, but it's probably too condensed to provide any very good sense of my argument.

Martin Taylor's contribution to the LCS IV volume may also be of interest to you when that book appears. I've seen some early drafts of his chapters, and many of his ideas were immediately influential to me as I put my own chapter together. He comes at the question of how to apply PCT to social interaction from a somewhat different angle than I do, but his work is unlike anything else I've seen, and I think it will prove foundational for lots of future applications of PCT in this area.

Please send me some examples of your work if there are things you'd like to share. I'm definitely interested.

And thanks to all for the birthday greetings! I've had a fun birthday going with two of my young grandsons to the chocolate museum in Hamburg, Germany.

Kent

On Sep 24, 2017, at 3:08 PM, Christopher Paul Geddes <<mailto:geddes.chris@gmail.com>geddes.chris@gmail.com> wrote:

Angus,

Thanks for writing. I hope all is well. I have done some modeling in the past however I was kind of limited and now I am more focused on Systems Dynamics which is making me take more of an interest in modeling now as well.

I would like to know more about what you are looking for if I can help. I like to think of organizations as entities that I can influence with technology so that things so the way the team wants them to go.

Christopher

On Sun, Sep 24, 2017 at 5:48 AM, Angus Jenkinson <<mailto:angus@angusjenkinson.com>angus@angusjenkinson.com> wrote:

[Angus Jenkinson, 2017-09-23.11.46]

Kent, Bruce Nevin has suggested that I should reach out to you in relationship to some work on modelling that you have been doing on collective behaviour. I am interested, and working with, the behaviour of organisations and social groups and the relationship between the individual and the group. I understand you have done some computer modelling of this. Is there something you can share?

And happy birthday!

…¦………………………………………€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦â€¦………….
Angus Jenkinson

--
Regards,

Christopher Paul Geddes

Email: <mailto:geddes.chris@gmx.com>geddes.chris@gmail.com
Mobile: +1-876-393-1366

Angus Jenkinson, 29.12.2017

Bruce
Thank you for once again seeking to advise me. I am responding to the passage quoted below from you responding to my questions/comments.

I am directly familiar with the mathematical intuitions you mention. And Peirce’s argument is in a tradition that includes Whewell’s discovery of the element of intuition and pattern forming that prefaces all scientific theory.

However I am also familiar with the same kind of intuition (let us call it) that prefaces the struggle to express itself in words. Both cases are codified means of expressing pure thinking. Such thinking is possible beyond or without language but cannot
be communicated. Either mathematics only thinks in a restricted domain or its expression in present mathematical possibilities restricts its ability to convey the full complexity of “reality�. Because all the mathematics today cannot capture all the world
of hearing a lark’s song.

Take the concept that ran Russell and Whitehead aground. The set of all sets that does not contain itself. I can think that mathematically from that notation.

I contend that mathematics is the best way of conveying various realms of explanation and insight, as music is for others, and so forth. It is a mighty achievement but still an unfinished project (I hope). Some things I love about mathematics include the
ability to produce a logic you would not think and have to puzzle over after, the dynamic quality it never loses, inviting process, and the sparkling clear beauty it often offers.

On the alerting, you do not answer the question. Or if you intend to I find it interesting.

The models are basic and too simple to express reality, says Kent. As models they are patterns. But the alerting is to something more than the model shows, he says. Thus the paper has both mathematics and verbal discussion. The verbal says, and model design
implies, that an extrapolation is possible. But the model cannot show what the full pattern would reveal. (It would be another discussion to point out the danger of extrapolating beyond what the model actually models.). If that alerting is to be understood
in a useful way, but the mathematical extrapolation has not been done, then I asked in what language the alerting takes place. Do you say, No language, pure thought pattern?

Angus Jenkinson
Eco Thinking

···

On 26 Dec 2017, at 16:15, Bruce Nevin bnhpct@gmail.com wrote:

Mathematics: the patterns, relationships, etc. so notationally represented are perceptions independent of language. An experienced mathematician knows the challenge of putting a mathematical ‘intuition’ into demonstrable form. You may recall how C.S. Peirce
inveighed against the tidy, pyramidal proof, preferring an account of how the mathematician actually arrived at his or her conclusion.

KM: What mathematical models are good for, in my view, is to alert us to the basic patterns of dynamic interaction…

The ‘alerting’ is in the form of mathematical perceptions of “the basic patterns of dynamic interaction”, facilitated or sometimes merely hinted by the language-like notational expressions with which a mathematical model is presented.

[From Bruce Nevin (2017.12.31.23:25 ET)]

Kent McClelland (2017.10.09.1500 EDT) –

Angus Jenkinson, 26.12.17. 15:00 Singapore –

KM: What mathematical models are good for, in my view, is to alert us to the basic patterns of dynamic interaction…<

Â

AJ: If a [mathematical] model is required to provide a rigorous analysis and yet the complexity of shared cooperative/conflict reality is too dense to model, in practice, how is the subject to be understood?

AJ: If mathematics is a form of language, as some argue, would it make more sense to say that is hard to achieve any description that does justice to the complexity of social interaction?

AJ: And in what language or thinking form does the “alertingâ€? take placeâ€?

Angus Jenkinson, 29.12.2017 –

AJ: On the alerting, you do not answer the question. Or if you intend to I find it interesting.Â

AJ:Â The models are basic and too simple to express reality, says Kent. As models they are patterns. But the alerting is to something more than the model shows, he says. Thus the paper has both mathematics and verbal discussion. The verbal says, and model design implies, that an extrapolation is possible. But the model cannot show what the full pattern would reveal. (It would be another discussion to point out the danger of extrapolating beyond what the model actually models.). If that alerting is to be understood in a useful way, but the mathematical extrapolation has not been done, then I asked in what language the alerting takes place. Do you say, No language, pure thought pattern?

But I did answer, as follows:

KM: What mathematical models are good for, in my view, is to alert us to the basic patterns of dynamic interaction…/blockquote>

BN: The ‘alerting’ is in the form of mathematical perceptions of “the basic patterns of dynamic interaction”, facilitated or sometimes merely hinted by the language-like notational expressions with which a mathematical model is presented.

Language is a rather rigid channel for transmission of information, and as its derivative (specialized for mathematical perceptions) so likewise are the various mathematical notations and the language expressions in which mathematicians ‘read out’ such notations.

The transmission is by what we might describe as a socially structured evocation of memory associations.Â

As I see it, there are associations linking language and non-language perceptions. Rather than Category perceptions, I suspect (and have proposed) that the associative links are probably perceptual inputs (both language inputs of non-language perceptions and non-language inputs from language perceptions) as well as associative memory. Bill proposed that the nexus of the perceptual input links was in perceptual input functions at the Category level. I have written about my misgivings, but the proposals I have made are also speculative and undemonstrated. Anyway, Bill assumed that the language input to a category would be a word, but the associations are not just between individual words and non-language perceptions, but also involve on the language side word-dependencies (between an operator word and particular words in its argument, constructions of greater or lesser complexity, fixed expressions, etc.

AJ: Either mathematics only thinks in a restricted domain or its expression in present mathematical possibilities restricts its ability to convey the full complexity of “realityâ€?. Because all the mathematics today cannot capture all the world of hearing a lark’s song.Â

I can’t speak to limitations of mathematics, but we surely don’t know all of it any more than we know all of the reality that we attempt to model with mathematical models. And I think what you mean by the world of hearing a lark’s song has as much in it of human emotional response as it does of larks, or am I mistaken? We cannot ‘capture’ all of our experience in any form of language or language-like system. How much we ‘capture’ is up to the recipient as much as it is up to the speaker or writer. And that is true even of those who write to find out what they think (as some have said). An interesting reflection, that.

To paraphrase Korzybski’s pithy aphorism the model is not the phenomenon. And to paraphrase an equally pithy aphorism of Sapir, all models leak. But by understanding (our perceptions of) the model we can more alertly perceive phenomena that are more complex than is convenient to be modeled. But I am only repeating here your paraphrase of Kent: “The models are basic and too simple to express reality”. Rarely, I think, does a model or an experiment not rest on some prior selection of cases amenable to be considered.

That is in answer to your question

AJ: If a [mathematical] model is required to provide a rigorous analysis and yet the complexity of shared cooperative/conflict reality is too dense to model, in practice, how is the subject to be understood?
Â

We must not forget that the models, the mathematical expressions, the language expressions, the social interactions that Kent is beginning to model, and the larks with their songs, are not accessible to us in their full reality, only as perceptions, however extended our native perceptions may be by the perceptions that we control as instruments and by the perceptions that we control as models.

You opened your email with:

AJ:Â Thank you for once again seeking to advise me.Â

Oh my. I’m honored that you think so (though the words from another might be mocking). Actually, I’m just trying to articulate my understandings, putting them out there to be corrected if I misunderstand or amended if there’s a better way of articulating them. So please fire away.

Happy new year, all! May you control well all the perceptions that most matter to you.

···

On Sat, Dec 30, 2017 at 9:30 AM, Angus Jenkinson angus@angusjenkinson.com wrote:

Angus Jenkinson, 29.12.2017

Bruce
Thank you for once again seeking to advise me. I am responding to the passage quoted below from you responding to my questions/comments.Â

I am directly familiar with the mathematical intuitions you mention. And Peirce’s argument is in a tradition that includes Whewell’s discovery of the element of intuition and pattern forming that prefaces all scientific theory.Â

However I am also familiar with the same kind of intuition (let us call it) that prefaces the struggle to express itself in words. Both cases are codified means of expressing pure thinking. Such thinking is possible beyond or without language but cannot
be communicated. Either mathematics only thinks in a restricted domain or its expression in present mathematical possibilities restricts its ability to convey the full complexity of “realityâ€?. Because all the mathematics today cannot capture all the world
of hearing a lark’s song.Â

Take the concept that ran Russell and Whitehead aground. The set of all sets that does not contain itself. I can think that mathematically from that notation.Â

I contend that mathematics is the best way of conveying various realms of explanation and insight, as music is for others, and so forth. It is a mighty achievement but still an unfinished project (I hope). Some things I love about mathematics include the
ability to produce a logic you would not think and have to puzzle over after, the dynamic quality it never loses, inviting process, and the sparkling clear beauty it often offers.Â

On the alerting, you do not answer the question. Or if you intend to I find it interesting.Â

The models are basic and too simple to express reality, says Kent. As models they are patterns. But the alerting is to something more than the model shows, he says. Thus the paper has both mathematics and verbal discussion. The verbal says, and model design
implies, that an extrapolation is possible. But the model cannot show what the full pattern would reveal. (It would be another discussion to point out the danger of extrapolating beyond what the model actually models.). If that alerting is to be understood
in a useful way, but the mathematical extrapolation has not been done, then I asked in what language the alerting takes place. Do you say, No language, pure thought pattern?

Angus Jenkinson
Eco Thinking

On 26 Dec 2017, at 16:15, Bruce Nevin bnhpct@gmail.com wrote:

Mathematics: the patterns, relationships, etc. so notationally represented are perceptions independent of language. An experienced mathematician knows the challenge of putting a mathematical ‘intuition’ into demonstrable form. You may recall how C.S. Peirce
inveighed against the tidy, pyramidal proof, preferring an account of how the mathematician actually arrived at his or her conclusion.

KM: What mathematical models are good for, in my view, is to alert us to the basic patterns of dynamic interaction

The ‘alerting’ is in the form of mathematical perceptions of “the basic patterns of dynamic interaction”, facilitated or sometimes merely hinted by the language-like notational expressions with which a mathematical model is presented.