Elements in the PCT model

[From Bjorn Simonsen (2006.11.24,23:45 EUST)]

From Bill
Powers (2006.11.24.1045 MST)]

You guys are still talking about different things.
Bjorn says that

light is a disturbance. You say it is not. So you
are talking

completely past each other.

You are a smart guy, Bill. And smart guys often
understand what they read. I experience often the problem that I misunderstand
what I read. If I get it explained in another way I often experience things
better.

I don’t know how Martin feels my questions, but he has
not stopped me asking and I appreciate his patience.

I don’t feel we talk past each other. But I may be
wrong.

In your example below I had overseen that Martin said:
“Changes in the knob angle cause (note “cause” rather than
“influence”, because I’m not even considering the possible existence
of a disturbance at this point).”

I didn’t read that he did not considered any
disturbance at this moment, and I had your Live Block with a disturbance in my
head.

Bjorn:

The
loop can be presented as a composed loop, as a loop

expressing what happens when we wish to
perceive something or what

happens when we are imputed a disturbance
e.g. in form of a light.

Martin:

I made a point that the loop I was considering was
NOT subject to

ANY disturbance. The error in the starting
condition came from the

fact that the screen was showing a colout (say
red, for example)

different from the reference colour (blue, you
suggest).>>>

If I ask too much in a wrong way, please tell me. I
think Martin and I agree at last. But he has very much to give still I think.

Bjorn

[Martin Taylor 2006.11.24.20.26]

[From Fred Nickols (2006.11.24.1431 EST)] --

If I'm trying to look at something and a bright light is causing me to squint or shade my eyes, it seems to me that this bright light is a disturbance to my goal of vewing something. On the other hand, if the light doesn't interfere with my ability to do that, then it's not a disturbance.

Do I have the correct?

Quite succinct. Yes.

Martin

[From Rick Marken (2006.11.24.2320)]

Martin Taylor (2006.11.24.20.26)

Fred Nickols (2006.11.24.1431 EST)] --

If I'm trying to look at something and a bright light is causing me to squint or shade my eyes, it seems to me that this bright light is a disturbance to my goal of vewing something. On the other hand, if the light doesn't interfere with my ability to do that, then it's not a disturbance.

Do I have the correct?

Quite succinct. Yes.

Succinct, yes, but not correct in the PCT sense of disturbance as "the variable that is responsible for the change in the input quantity". Fred is describing a disturbance as a change in the input quantity that is caused by some other unnamed variable. This is not the PCT meaning of disturbance.

The "something" Fred is looking at is reflected light from some object. The reflected light is an input quantity. When this input quantity is in stare x it is seen as "something", which is presumably the desired state of the input . The bright light represents a change in the input quantity -- when the value of this quantity goes way above x. Squinting is the output that brings the input quantity back toward x. When there is no "interfering" bright light there is no "disturbance" in the sense that there is no change in the input quantity; it remains at x.

  But from the PCT perspective, the disturbance is the variable that causes the brightening of the input variable (the light reflected into the eye from the object out there). So the disturbance variable is the source of the light being reflected off the "something" Say we're outside during the day so the main light source is light from the sky and this source varies depending on the sun's location in the sky and the cloudiness of the sky. So "light source" is the disturbance variable and it is a disturbance (in the PCT sense) whether the source is bright or hazy. Variations in the light source disturbance cause variations in the input quantity -- reflected light -- even when the momentary result of this disturbance is no change in the input away from the value x.

Disturbances, in the PCT sense of "variables responsible for changes (variations) in an input quantity" are always there. They are not sometimes a disturbance and other times not.

Best

Rick

···

---

Richard S. Marken Consulting
marken@mindreadings.com
Home 310 474-0313
Cell 310 729-1400

[From Rick Marken (2006.11.25.0850)]

Martin Taylor (2006.11.25.10.28)

From Rick Marken (2006.11.24.2320)

Martin Taylor (2006.11.24.20.26)

Fred Nickols (2006.11.24.1431 EST) --

If I'm trying to look at something and a bright light is causing me to squint or shade my eyes, it seems to me that this bright light is a disturbance to my goal of vewing something. On the other hand, if the light doesn't interfere with my ability to do that, then it's not a disturbance.

Do I have the correct?

Quite succinct. Yes.

Succinct, yes, but not correct in the PCT sense of disturbance as "the variable that is responsible for the change in the input quantity".

Not for the first time, I'm afraid I have to disagree with Rick.

And not for the last time I'm afraid you're wrong (well, I'm not that afraid;-))

Fred (as I interpret him) is talking about a control loop controlling for seeing some object. Before the "bright light" came on, he could see it well. When the light came on, he could see it less well -- maybe he couldn't even see it at all. So the control system controlling his perception of how well he could see produced output to counter the effect of the bright light. To me, that shows that the bright light constituted a disturbance. The example correctly and succinctly illustrates the meaning of the word.

Then how do you explain Fred's last point: "if the light doesn't interfere with my ability to do that, then it's not a disturbance"? If the bright light were a disturbance in the PCT sense of "disturbing variable" then it's still a disturbance even when it doesn't interfere with the ability to see the object.

  I think Fred is clearly describing a "disturbance" in the non-PCT sense, as a change in the input variable (the consequence of variation in a disturbing variable). I was trying to explain that this is not a "disturbance" in the PCT sense; it is simply variation in the input variable, the cause of which (in a normal control loop) is always variation in simultaneously the disturbance (PCT sense of disturbing variable) and the output variable. The disturbance (PCT sense) in Fred's example would be a variable light source that contributes to the brightness of the reflections that sometimes interfere with the desired perception of the input quantity, and sometimes don't, depending on the current value of the disturbing variable

This is technically correct (though see below), provided one keeps in mind a point that is often missed in talking about control: disturbances can be there with magnitude zero.

Right. Disturbances, in the PCT sense of "disturbing variables", are there no matter what there value. This is what I was keeping in mind. You should too;-)

Best

Rick

Richard S. Marken Consulting
marken@mindreadings.com
Home 310 474-0313
Cell 310 729-1400

[Martin Taylor 2006.11.25.12.37]

[From Rick Marken (2006.11.25.0850)]

Martin Taylor (2006.11.25.10.28)

From Rick Marken (2006.11.24.2320)

Martin Taylor (2006.11.24.20.26)

Fred Nickols (2006.11.24.1431 EST) --

If I'm trying to look at something and a bright light is causing me to squint or shade my eyes, it seems to me that this bright light is a disturbance to my goal of vewing something. On the other hand, if the light doesn't interfere with my ability to do that, then it's not a disturbance.

Do I have the correct?

Quite succinct. Yes.

Succinct, yes, but not correct in the PCT sense of disturbance as "the variable that is responsible for the change in the input quantity".

Not for the first time, I'm afraid I have to disagree with Rick.

And not for the last time I'm afraid you're wrong (well, I'm not that afraid;-))

Fred (as I interpret him) is talking about a control loop controlling for seeing some object. Before the "bright light" came on, he could see it well. When the light came on, he could see it less well -- maybe he couldn't even see it at all. So the control system controlling his perception of how well he could see produced output to counter the effect of the bright light. To me, that shows that the bright light constituted a disturbance. The example correctly and succinctly illustrates the meaning of the word.

Then how do you explain Fred's last point: "if the light doesn't interfere with my ability to do that, then it's not a disturbance"?

He is correct. It isn't, if it causes a change only in an uncontrolled perception. That's why his example is a good one. In his scenario, if switching on the bright light doesn't cause his "seeing it well" control system's output to change, then it clearly isn't a disturbance to that control system.

If the bright light were a disturbance in the PCT sense of "disturbing variable" then it's still a disturbance even when it doesn't interfere with the ability to see the object.

This is a very confusing (or confused?) statement. If something is to qualify as a disturbing variable, a change in it must be associated with a countering change in the control system's output. If the system is controlling for "ability to see the object" and an external event (switching on the bright light) doesn't interfere with the ability to see the object, then it isn't a disturbance to that control system (though it may well disturb other control systems).

Martin

[From Rick Marken (2006.11.25.1040)]

Martin Taylor (2006.11.25.12.37)

From Rick Marken (2006.11.25.0850)

Then how do you explain Fred's last point: "if the light doesn't interfere with my ability to do that, then it's not a disturbance"?

He is correct. It isn't, if it causes a change only in an uncontrolled perception. That's why his example is a good one. In his scenario, if switching on the bright light doesn't cause his "seeing it well" control system's output to change, then it clearly isn't a disturbance to that control system.

Ah. I see. You interpreted the statement to be something like "if the light has no influence on the input quantity, regardless of its value (bright or not bright) then it is not a disturbing variable". So would you agree, then, that the source of the bright light that interferes with seeing the object is still a disturbance when the light is no longer an "interference".

If the bright light were a disturbance in the PCT sense of "disturbing variable" then it's still a disturbance even when it doesn't interfere with the ability to see the object.

This is a very confusing (or confused?) statement. If something is to qualify as a disturbing variable, a change in it must be associated with a countering change in the control system's output. If the system is controlling for "ability to see the object" and an external event (switching on the bright light) doesn't interfere with the ability to see the object, then it isn't a disturbance to that control system (though it may well disturb other control systems).

I don't understand why you find this confusing.

Let's take the example of driving a car. When there is no cross wind, the wind does not interfere with the direction of the car on the road (the controlled input). But the wind is still a disturbance variable, because it is an independent and VARIABLE_ influence on value of the input variable. The wind (like the brightness of the light in Fred's example) is a disturbance variable. It is still a disturbance, whether the value of this variable is 100 mph coming a right angle to the direction of movement of the car or zero -- so that it is, for the moment at least, not an "interference" in the sense that it is not "pushing" or "pulling" the perception of the car's position on the road -- the input quantity -- away from the reference state.

How about working a couple of my example problems so I can see if your concept of a disturbance is the same as mine. Let's start with "Watching a distant sailboat on the sea", which is similar to Fred's example. What's the input quantity, output variable, disturbance variable?

Best

Rick

···

----
Richard S. Marken Consulting
marken@mindreadings.com
Home 310 474-0313
Cell 310 729-1400

[From Bill Powers (2006.11.25.1106 MST)]

Martin Taylor 2006.11.25.12.37 –

Rick:

Then how do you explain Fred’s
last point: “if the light doesn’t interfere with my ability to do
that, then it’s not a disturbance”?

Martin:

He is correct. It isn’t, if it causes a change only in an uncontrolled
perception. That’s why his example is a good one. In his scenario, if
switching on the bright light doesn’t cause his “seeing it
well” control system’s output to change, then it clearly isn’t a
disturbance to that control system.

One problem is still an ambiguity in the meanings of words. It doesn’t
help if you switch from “disturb” to “interfere” if
it’s still not clear whether you’re talking about the interfering cause
or the interference it produces. A bright light can interfere with seeing
something else, without interfering with seeing the something else. That
is, the bright light tends to reduce the visibility of the
something else, but the squinting mostly counteracts that tendency so you
can still see the something else. The interference doesn’t interfere with
seeing the something else. The action of the control system reduces the
amount of interference caused by the interfering variable to the point
where you are not prevented from seeing the something else.

Another problem is that of using an approximation to convert a
quantitative statement into a qualitatative one. The effect of the action
of a control system is [approximately] equal and opposite to the effect
of the disturbance on the input quantity. If you leave out the word
[approximately], then you can give the qualitative impression that the
disturbance has no effect at all on the controlled quantity, which cannot
ever be true. Even integrators are imperfect in a real system, so
[approximately] integrating output functions have finite gain and the
error can never go exactly to zero. Disturbing variables always have an
effect on the input quantity. It’s small, but it exists. If it didn’t,
there would be no control.

Rick:

If the bright light were a
disturbance in the PCT sense of “disturbing variable” then it’s
still a disturbance even when it doesn’t interfere with the ability to
see the object.

It’s still a physical variable, to be sure, but if it doesn’t cause a
[small] change in some controlled variable, it’s not a disturbing
variable.

All you guys have to be more aware of alternate ways of reading what you
write. You know what you mean because you start with the meaning you want
in your mind and think you are putting it into words because you read
your own words with the desired meaning in mind. But if someone else
finds an obvious different way to read what you write, you haven’t
succeeded. You can’t say what you mean until you eliminate the obvious
alternate meanings.

Martin:

If something is to qualify as a
disturbing variable, a change in it must be associated with a countering
change in the control system’s output.

A “countering” change? How about “a change that almost
completely counters the effect of the disturbing variable?” That
“almost” is important.

If the system is
controlling for “ability to see the object” and an external
event (switching on the bright light) doesn’t interfere with the ability
to see the object, then it isn’t a disturbance to that control system
(though it may well disturb other control systems).

This is not true. The bright light might not cause interference with
seeing the object for the simple reason that the action of the control
system (squinting) reduces the amount of interference nearly to
zero.

Which end of the arrow are you talking about?

(magnitude
of
(net effect on

disturbing
variable)
input quantity)

Disturbance ----------------------> disturbance [<------ from
qo]

Interference
interference if controlled

(cause)
(effect)

Best,

Bill P.

[From Bill Powers (2006.11.25.1202 MST)]

Isn't this clearer than all those words?

                                                     high
                                                 visibility
                                                      >
                                    --------------->Comp -------->
                                   > >
Brightness of light ----->visibility of object <------------ degree of squint

Best,

Bill P.

[From Fred Nickols (2006.11.25.1431 EST)] --

[From Bill Powers (2006.11.25.1202 MST)]

Isn't this clearer than all those words?

                                                     high
                                                 visibility
                                                      >
                                    --------------->Comp -------->
                                   > >
                                   > >
Brightness of light ----->visibility of object <------------ degree of squint

Best,

Bill P.

It is for me and it captures precisely what I was getting at - I think. :frowning:

Let's assume a starting point with all elements present. The value of brightness is 1. With that level of brightness, I don't need to squint and I see the object just fine. The brightness of the light is not a disturbance. If the brightness exceeds 1, the object becomes more difficult to see but I can offset it by squinting. The value of squint goes up to offset a value of brightness greater than 1. The brightness is a disturbance but a manageable one. Ditto for decreasing the brightness of the light. At some point it's so dark I can't see the object and no amount of squinting will help. Again, brightness is a disturbance but because it's too little, not too much. So, I turn on the ceiling light and everything's fine. :slight_smile:

Regards,

Fred Nickols
nickols@att.net

[Martin Taylor 2006.11.25.14.53]

[From Bill Powers (2006.11.25.1106 MST)]

Martin Taylor 2006.11.25.12.37 --

Rick:

Then how do you explain Fred's last point: "if the light doesn't interfere with my ability to do that, then it's not a disturbance"?

Martin:
He is correct. It isn't, if it causes a change only in an uncontrolled perception. That's why his example is a good one. In his scenario, if switching on the bright light doesn't cause his "seeing it well" control system's output to change, then it clearly isn't a disturbance to that control system.

One problem is still an ambiguity in the meanings of words. It doesn't help if you switch from "disturb" to "interfere" if it's still not clear whether you're talking about the interfering cause or the interference it produces.

I think you have to bend over backwards to find the ambiguity in what I wrote. I guess it's good exercise, though. But it's true, when dealing with language, ambiguity can almost always be found if you look hard enough.

If the system is controlling for "ability to see the object" and an external event (switching on the bright light) doesn't interfere with the ability to see the object, then it isn't a disturbance to that control system (though it may well disturb other control systems).

This is not true. The bright light might not cause interference with seeing the object for the simple reason that the action of the control system (squinting) reduces the amount of interference nearly to zero.

Which end of the arrow are you talking about?

(magnitude of (net effect on
disturbing variable) input quantity)

Disturbance ----------------------> disturbance [<------ from qo]
Interference interference if controlled
(cause) (effect)

I'm pointing out that it is NOT the effect on the input quantity that matters. You get an effect on the input quantity whether the perceptual variable is controlled or not. But if whatever changes doesn't affect soem output quantity, then the variable that changed is not a disturbance to whatever perceptual variable is associated with that output quantity.

Try this:

Magnitude of Magnitude of Magnitude of
change in change in change in
disturbing --------------> input <------------------- output
variable quantity quantity

Delta d delta qi delta qo

If Delta d >> epsilon (where epsilon is an small but detectable value), then if qo >> 0, and qo is opposite in sign from d, then d is a disturbance. (delta qi is likely to be small, but could be large if control is poor. The size of the change in qi is totally irrelevant. Of course, if it is zero, there's no disturbance).

Else if Delta d >>, and qo ~= 0 or qo > 0 but is uncorrelated with delta d, then d is not a disturbance.

I know the ambiguity in saying "uncorrelated", because it takes infinite time to remove all correlation. But I think you ought to allow an assumption of the word "almost" when you know that all concerned are aware of it.

Martin

[From Rick Marken (2006.11.25.1330)]

Fred Nickols (2006.11.25.1431 EST) --

Bill Powers (2006.11.25.1202 MST)]

Isn't this clearer than all those words?

                                                     high
                                                 visibility
                                                      >
                                    --------------->Comp -------->
                                   > >
Brightness of light ----->visibility of object <------------ degree of squint

It is for me and it captures precisely what I was getting at - I think. :frowning:

Let's assume a starting point with all elements present. The value of brightness is 1. With that level of brightness, I don't need to squint and I see the object just fine. The brightness of the light is not a disturbance.

What?!?! You mean the brightness of light leaves the diagram when its value is 1.0? I think you are still using the word "disturbance" to refer to a change in the input quantity (visibility of object, in this case) away from the reference state. The brightness of light in this diagram is _not_ a disturbance in that sense. It is a disturbance VARiABLE that is always there, variably influencing the visibility of the object.

If the brightness exceeds 1, the object becomes more difficult to see but I can offset it by squinting. The value of squint goes up to offset a value of brightness greater than 1. The brightness is a disturbance but a manageable one.

The brightness of light does not leave the diagram (as a disturbance) when its value is 1 and then come back onto the diagram when it's at a "manageable" value. The only "disturbance" that acts that way is the potential change in the input quantity that could be produced by variations in the brightness of light.

I think you've proven that the diagram is not necessarily any clearer than words. The diagram (to me) shows that the brightness of light is a disturbance VARIABLE which influences the input quantity, visibility of the object. It is a disturbance (in the PCT sense of disturbance variable) whether its value is 0, 1 or 1,000.

Best

Rick

···

----
Richard S. Marken Consulting
marken@mindreadings.com
Home 310 474-0313
Cell 310 729-1400

[From Bill Powers (2006.11.25.1430 MST)]

Martin Taylor 2006.11.25.14.53 --

I'm pointing out that it is NOT the effect on the input quantity that matters. You get an effect on the input quantity whether the perceptual variable is controlled or not. But if whatever changes doesn't affect soem output quantity, then the variable that changed is not a disturbance to whatever perceptual variable is associated with that output quantity.

Try this:

Magnitude of Magnitude of Magnitude of
change in change in change in
disturbing --------------> input <------------------- output
variable quantity quantity

Delta d delta qi delta qo

If Delta d >> epsilon (where epsilon is an small but detectable value), then if qo >> 0, and qo is opposite in sign from d, then d is a disturbance. (delta qi is likely to be small, but could be large if control is poor. The size of the change in qi is totally irrelevant. Of course, if it is zero, there's no disturbance).

Else if Delta d >>, and qo ~= 0 or qo > 0 but is uncorrelated with delta d, then d is not a disturbance.

OK, good points. If the disturbed variable is not under control, no change in output will occur. I'd still prefer to say that the input quantity is what is disturbed, but the sign that it is a controlled variable is the output response to the disturbance. And I think "response" is the right word here because of the automatic nature of the reaction (also the right word).

I once proposed that we use "perturbation" to refer to an actual small change in a quantity: the disturbing variable produces a perturbation of the controlled variable. The size of the perturbation depends also on the quality of control (speed and gain).

Best,

Bill P.

···

I know the ambiguity in saying "uncorrelated", because it takes infinite time to remove all correlation. But I think you ought to allow an assumption of the word "almost" when you know that all concerned are aware of it.

Martin

--
No virus found in this incoming message.
Checked by AVG Free Edition.
Version: 7.5.430 / Virus Database: 268.14.16/551 - Release Date: 11/25/2006 10:55 AM

[From Rick Marken (2006.11.25.1345)]

Martin Taylor (2006.11.25.14.53) --

Try this:

Magnitude of Magnitude of Magnitude of
change in change in change in
disturbing --------------> input <------------------- output
variable quantity quantity

Delta d delta qi delta qo

If Delta d >> epsilon (where epsilon is an small but detectable value), then if qo >> 0, and qo is opposite in sign from d, then d is a disturbance. (delta qi is likely to be small, but could be large if control is poor. The size of the change in qi is totally irrelevant. Of course, if it is zero, there's no disturbance).

Else if Delta d >>, and qo ~= 0 or qo > 0 but is uncorrelated with delta d, then d is not a disturbance.

This is not what "disturbance" means in PCT. In PCT, disturbance means: "Any variable in the environment of a control system that a) contributes to changes in the controlled input quantity and b) is not controlled by the same control system" (B:CP, 2E, p 296)

Best

Rick

···

---

Richard S. Marken Consulting
marken@mindreadings.com
Home 310 474-0313
Cell 310 729-1400

[From Bill Powers (2006.11.25.1440 MST)]

Rick Marken (2006.11.25.1330) --

(to Fred N.)I think you've proven that the diagram is not necessarily any clearer than words. The diagram (to me) shows that the brightness of light is a disturbance VARIABLE which influences the input quantity, visibility of the object. It is a disturbance (in the PCT sense of disturbance variable) whether its value is 0, 1 or 1,000.

Yes, I agree. "Disturbing variable" is the name of some physical variable. Calling it a "disturbing" variable implies that we have found that a control system reacts to it by opposing its effect on some other variable. The value of the disturbing variable can be anything -- positive or negative by any amount, or zero. It's the same principle as calling the indicator on a car's dashboard a "speedometer" even if it happens to be displaying zero speed. A speedometer reading of zero is still a reading. A disturbing variable with a value of zero is still a disturbng variable because of its relationship to a control system.

This problem may be part of the legacy of stimulus-response psychology, in which both stimuli and responses are usually thought of as "events", the fifth order of perception. Maybe this is why there is a tendency to speak of disturbances as either happening or not happening, instead of being present in varying degrees. There are, I have proposed, ten other kinds of variables that can be disturbed and controlled by the actions of a control system. A steady force is a disturbance when force-intensity is being controlled. An unauthorized withdrawal from a checking account is a perturbation at a higher level due to someone with joint access to the same bank account.

Well, we've about beaten this one to death. Maybe it will stop staggering to its feet again when we turn our backs.

Best,

Bill P.

[From Bill Powers (2006.11.25.1455 MST)]

Rick Marken (2006.11.25.1345) --

This is not what "disturbance" means in PCT. In PCT, disturbance means: "Any variable in the environment of a control system that a) contributes to changes in the controlled input quantity and b) is not controlled by the same control system" (B:CP, 2E, p 296)

I think Martin has given us an operational definition of a disturbing variable or a disturbance. The opposing action of the control system is a valid sign that a disturbance has been applied to a controlled variable. If no opposing action occurs, the variable that was disturbed was not a controlled variable.

Does that check out for you?

Best,

Bill P.

[From Rick Marken (2006.11.25.1420)]

Bill Powers (2006.11.25.1455 MST)

Rick Marken (2006.11.25.1345) --

This is not what "disturbance" means in PCT. In PCT, disturbance means: "Any variable in the environment of a control system that a) contributes to changes in the controlled input quantity and b) is not controlled by the same control system" (B:CP, 2E, p 296)

I think Martin has given us an operational definition of a disturbing variable or a disturbance. The opposing action of the control system is a valid sign that a disturbance has been applied to a controlled variable. If no opposing action occurs, the variable that was disturbed was not a controlled variable.

Does that check out for you?

Not quite. Here's Martin's "operational definition".

Magnitude of Magnitude of Magnitude of
change in change in change in
disturbing --------------> input <------------------- output
variable quantity quantity

Delta d delta qi delta qo

If Delta d >> epsilon (where epsilon is an small but detectable value), then if qo >> 0, and qo is opposite in sign from d, then d is a disturbance. (delta qi is likely to be small, but could be large if control is poor. The size of the change in qi is totally irrelevant. Of course, if it is zero, there's no disturbance).

Else if Delta d >>, and qo ~= 0 or qo > 0 but is uncorrelated with delta d, then d is not a disturbance.

The problem is that the value of qo could be > 0 or 0 due to changes in the reference signal. So this s -> r or delta d -> qo approach to operationalizing a disturbance will only work if you assume that the reference signal is a constant, which is kind of a questionable assumption, even when you ask people to maintain a fixed goal.

Your operational definition of a disturbance still works best for me.

Best

Rick

···

---
Richard S. Marken Consulting
marken@mindreadings.com
Home 310 474-0313
Cell 310 729-1400

[From Rick Marken (2006.11.25.1420)]

Bill Powers (2006.11.25.1455 MST)

I think Martin has given us an operational definition of a disturbing variable or a disturbance. The opposing action of the control system is a valid sign that a disturbance has been applied to a controlled variable. If no opposing action occurs, the variable that was disturbed was not a controlled variable.

Does that check out for you?

Not quite. Here's Martin's "operational definition".

Magnitude of Magnitude of Magnitude of
change in change in change in
disturbing --------------> input <------------------- output
variable quantity quantity

Delta d delta qi delta qo

If Delta d >> epsilon (where epsilon is an small but detectable value), then if qo >> 0, and qo is opposite in sign from d, then d is a disturbance. (delta qi is likely to be small, but could be large if control is poor. The size of the change in qi is totally irrelevant. Of course, if it is zero, there's no disturbance).

Else if Delta d >>, and qo ~= 0 or qo > 0 but is uncorrelated with delta d, then d is not a disturbance.

The problem is that the value of qo could be > 0 or 0 due to changes in the reference signal.

Yes. I included that possibility by saying: "or qo > 0 but is uncorrelated with delta d". I was referring to the possibility of reference changes, and of coincidental changes in some other variable that really is disturbing the control system.

So this s -> r or delta d -> qo approach to operationalizing a disturbance will only work if you assume that the reference signal is a constant,

Not so. It's quite possible that the reference signal for control system C will on some occasion change coincidentally with a perceptible environmental event, and that will make the output of C change. But if the environmental envent is not actually a change in a disturbing variable of C, subsequent changes in the putative disturbing variable will not bear the same relationship to changes in the output of C. The correlation, over time, will tend to zero.

Observation of one event only gives a high probability that the environmental variable is or is not disturbing the control system C. Coincidental variation in the reference signal is always possible. That's life.

which is kind of a questionable assumption, even when you ask people to maintain a fixed goal.

Your operational definition of a disturbance still works best for me.

If you consider only the definition: "In PCT, disturbance means: "Any variable in the environment of a control system that a) contributes to changes in the controlled input quantity and b) is not controlled by the same control system" (B:CP, 2E, p 296)", then you have to find another way of determining that the input quantity is in fact controlled. That's what my operational definition does.

Martin

[From Fred Nickols (2006.11.25.1930 EST)] --

Regarding Rick's post below...

Rick, I think you attach way too much significance to what I say. More specifically, I think you are treating what I say as having meaning in PCT terms. I'm not sure it does. I'm still fumbling and groping, trying to understand, so my utterances shouldn't be taken too seriously.

Regards,

Fred Nickols

···

[From Rick Marken (2006.11.25.1330)]

> Fred Nickols (2006.11.25.1431 EST) --
>
>> Bill Powers (2006.11.25.1202 MST)]
>>
>> Isn't this clearer than all those words?
>>
>> high
>> visibility
>> >
>> --------------->Comp -------->
>> > >
>> > >
>> Brightness of light ----->visibility of object <------------ degree
>> of squint
>
> It is for me and it captures precisely what I was getting at - I
> think. :frowning:
>
> Let's assume a starting point with all elements present. The value of
> brightness is 1. With that level of brightness, I don't need to
> squint and I see the object just fine. The brightness of the light is
> not a disturbance.

What?!?! You mean the brightness of light leaves the diagram when its
value is 1.0? I think you are still using the word "disturbance" to
refer to a change in the input quantity (visibility of object, in this
case) away from the reference state. The brightness of light in this
diagram is _not_ a disturbance in that sense. It is a disturbance
VARiABLE that is always there, variably influencing the visibility of
the object.

> If the brightness exceeds 1, the object becomes more difficult to see
> but I can offset it by squinting. The value of squint goes up to
> offset a value of brightness greater than 1. The brightness is a
> disturbance but a manageable one.

The brightness of light does not leave the diagram (as a disturbance)
when its value is 1 and then come back onto the diagram when it's at a
"manageable" value. The only "disturbance" that acts that way is the
potential change in the input quantity that could be produced by
variations in the brightness of light.

I think you've proven that the diagram is not necessarily any clearer
than words. The diagram (to me) shows that the brightness of light is
a disturbance VARIABLE which influences the input quantity, visibility
of the object. It is a disturbance (in the PCT sense of disturbance
variable) whether its value is 0, 1 or 1,000.

Best

Rick

----
Richard S. Marken Consulting
marken@mindreadings.com
Home 310 474-0313
Cell 310 729-1400

[From Rick Marken (2006.11.25.1720)]

Rick Marken (2006.11.25.1420)

Not quite. Here's Martin's "operational definition".

If Delta d >> epsilon (where epsilon is an small but detectable value), then if qo >> 0, and qo is opposite in sign from d, then d is a disturbance. (delta qi is likely to be small, but could be large if control is poor. The size of the change in qi is totally irrelevant. Of course, if it is zero, there's no disturbance).

Else if Delta d >>, and qo ~= 0 or qo > 0 but is uncorrelated with delta d, then d is not a disturbance.

The problem is that the value of qo could be > 0 or 0 due to changes in the reference signal.

Yes. I included that possibility by saying: "or qo > 0 but is uncorrelated with delta d".

If it's the correlation between qo and delta d that matters (and it is; if they are uncorrelated as you say then, indeed, d is not a disturbance) then I see no reason to add the constraints about delta d and qo. In fact, adding those constraints (delta d >>, and qo ~= 0 or qo > 0 ) is a mistake, as far as I can see.

So this s -> r or delta d -> qo approach to operationalizing a disturbance will only work if you assume that the reference signal is a constant,

Not so. It's quite possible that the reference signal for control system C will on some occasion change coincidentally with a perceptible environmental event, and that will make the output of C change. But if the environmental envent is not actually a change in a disturbing variable of C, subsequent changes in the putative disturbing variable will not bear the same relationship to changes in the output of C. The correlation, over time, will tend to zero.

Right. So your operational definition just has to talk about correlations between d and qo over time, not just the relationship between delta d and qo. Then it will work. If qo~=-d, so that the correlation between these two variables is large and negative, then d is probably a disturbance to qi.

Best

Rick

···

---
Richard S. Marken Consulting
marken@mindreadings.com
Home 310 474-0313
Cell 310 729-1400

[Martin Taylor 2006.11.25.22.55]

I hope this is the last one.

[From Rick Marken (2006.11.25.1720)]

If it's the correlation between qo and delta d that matters (and it is; if they are uncorrelated as you say then, indeed, d is not a disturbance) then I see no reason to add the constraints about delta d and qo. In fact, adding those constraints (delta d >>, and qo ~= 0 or qo > 0 ) is a mistake, as far as I can see.

Now I think we aren't very far apart, and perhaps I can close the gap entirely.

In my view, it's not a mistake. It's an approximation, a very good approximation, but an approximation nevertheless. If a putative disturbance is observed, and no contervailing action occurs, it's a pretty good bet that the putative disturbance wasn't a disturbance after all. It's possible, but highly unlikely, that at the same moment the reference value changed so as to maintain the error unchanged. So, failure to observe a compensatory output is a strong indication that the putative disturbance was not one.

On the other side, observation of output without a corresponding change in the putative disturbing variable carries with it a very strong suggestion that if the thing is a disturbance, it isn't the only one, or that the reference value changed.

Over a finite time, correlation between two independent signals is zero only by chance, but appreciable deviations from zero get less and less likely as time goes on.

As for demanding that delta d >> epsilon, all that says is that the observer should be able to see tha a possibly disturbing event might have happened. If the observer can't see a possibly disturbing event, no compensating output is likely to be observable either, so there's no evidence either way.

The real essential point, which you seem to be avoiding, is that "disturbance" is a concept that applies only to a controlled perception, not to external events that change an uncontrolled perception. The effective way to distinguish them is to see whether the conditions indicating control are satisfied. The putative disturbance is providing an instance of "the Test".

Martin